Skip navigation

Predicting Erroneous Convictions - A Social Science Approach to Miscarriages of Justice, Gould et al, 2013

Download original document:
Brief thumbnail
This text is machine-read, and may contain errors. Check the original document to verify accuracy.
The author(s) shown below used Federal funds provided by the U.S.
Department of Justice and prepared the following final report:

Document Title:

Predicting Erroneous Convictions: A Social
Science Approach to Miscarriages of Justice

Author(s):

Jon B. Gould, Julia Carrano, Richard Leo,
Joseph Young

Document No.:

241389

Date Received:

February 2013

Award Number:

2009-IJ-CX-4110

This report has not been published by the U.S. Department of Justice.
To provide better customer service, NCJRS has made this Federallyfunded grant report available electronically.

Opinions or points of view expressed are those
of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect
the official position or policies of the U.S.
Department of Justice.

Predicting Erroneous Convictions: A Social Science Approach
to Miscarriages of Justice
Jon B. Gould
American University
4400 Massachusetts Avenue, N.W.
Washington, D.C. 20016
gould@american.edu
202-885-6535 (phone)
202-885-6536 (fax)
Julia Carrano
American University
4400 Massachusetts Avenue, N.W.
Washington, D.C. 20016
carrano@american.edu
202-885-6421 (phone)
202-885-6536 (fax)
Richard Leo
University of San Francisco
School of Law
2130 Fulton Street
San Francisco, CA 94117
rleo@usfca.edu
415-661-0162 (phone)
415-661-0172 (fax)
Joseph Young
American University
4400 Massachusetts Avenue, N.W.
Washington, D.C. 20016
jyoung@american.edu
202-885-2618 (phone)
202-885-6536 (fax)
With
Katie Hail-Jares
Andrea Butler
Jaclyn Menditch
Truman Morrison
Renée Nicole Souris
American University

Kevin Maass
Erin Crites
Sarah Ohlsen
George Mason University

December 2012
This project was conducted under Grant No. 2009-IJ-CX-4110 awarded by the National Institute of Justice, Office
of Justice Programs, United States Department of Justice. Points of view in this document are solely those of the
authors and do not necessarily represent the official position or policies of the United States Government.

This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

ABSTRACT
The last thirty years have seen an enormous increase not only in the exonerations of
innocent defendants but also academic scholarship on erroneous convictions. This literature has
identified a number of common factors that appear frequently in erroneous conviction cases,
including forensic error, prosecutorial misconduct, false confessions, and eyewitness
misidentification. However, without a comparison or control group of cases, researchers risk
labeling these factors as “causes” of erroneous convictions when they may be merely correlates.
In fact, the only way to establish what causes an erroneous conviction is to understand which
factors are exclusive to erroneous convictions as against other sets of cases.
This approach has been taken by only a handful of scholars, all of whom have been
interested in what separates erroneous convictions from other convictions. Missing so far in the
literature is a study that asks how the criminal justice system identifies innocent defendants in
order to prevent erroneous convictions. What we want to know—and thus what dictated our
research strategy—is what factors are uniquely present in cases that lead the system to rightfully
acquit or dismiss charges against the innocent defendant (so-called “near misses”), which are not
present in cases that lead the system to erroneously convict the innocent. If we understand this,
then we are closer to comprehending what policy interventions can influence the justice system
to prevent future erroneous convictions.
Our study employed a mixed methods approach that involved both quantitative and
qualitative analysis. We began by identifying a set of 460 erroneous conviction and near miss
cases that met a stringent definition of innocence. We then researched and coded the cases along
a number of variables, including location effects, nature of the victim, nature of the defendant,
facts available to the police and prosecutor, quality of work by the criminal justice system, and
ii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

quality of work by the defense. The cases were subsequently analyzed using bivariate and
logistic regression techniques. With the assistance of an expert panel, we also explored the cases
from a qualitative perspective and examined the statistical results in light of this exploration.
The results indicate that 10 factors—the age and criminal history of the defendant, the
punitiveness of the state, Brady violations, forensic error, a weak defense and prosecution case, a
family defense witness, an inadvertent misidentification, and lying by a non-eyewitness—help
explain why an innocent defendant, once indicted, ends up erroneously convicted rather than
released. Other factors traditionally suggested as sources of erroneous convictions, including
false confessions, criminal justice official error, and race effects, appear in statistically similar
rates in both sets of cases; thus, they likely increase the chance that an innocent suspect will be
indicted but not the likelihood that the indictment will result in a conviction. Finally, our
qualitative review of the cases reveals how the statistically significant factors are connected and
exacerbated by tunnel vision, which prevents the system from self-correcting once an error is
made. In fact, tunnel vision provides a useful framework for understanding the larger systemwide failure that separates erroneous convictions from near misses.
Among the policy implications of our findings is that increased attention to the failing
dynamics of the criminal justice system, rather than simply isolated errors or causes, may lead to
better prevention of erroneous convictions. In addition, our results suggest that there should be
greater emphasis at all levels and on all sides of the criminal justice system, including police,
prosecutors, defense attorneys and judges, to analyze and learn from past mistakes before they
result in serious miscarriages of justice. To this end, we encourage continued research on near
misses among both practitioners and scholars.

iii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

TABLE OF CONTENTS
ABSTRACT .................................................................................................................................................. ii 
TABLE OF CONTENTS ............................................................................................................................. iv 
EXECUTIVE SUMMARY ......................................................................................................................... xi 
I. INTRODUCTION ..................................................................................................................................... 1 
I.A. Background ........................................................................................................................................ 1 
I. B. What Do We Know Now? Factors that Correlate with Erroneous Conviction................................. 7 
I.B.1 Mistaken Eyewitness Identification .............................................................................................. 7 
I.B.2 False Confessions ......................................................................................................................... 9 
I.B.3. Tunnel Vision ............................................................................................................................ 15 
I.B.4. Perjured Informant Testimony .................................................................................................. 16 
I.B.5. Forensic Error .......................................................................................................................... 16 
I.B.6. Prosecutorial Error ................................................................................................................... 19 
I.B.7 Inadequate Defense Representation ........................................................................................... 20 
I.B.8. Interrelated Themes .................................................................................................................. 20 
I.C. New Methods: Social Science Research on What Distinguishes Erroneous Convictions from Other
Cases ....................................................................................................................................................... 23 
I.C.1. Social Science Approaches ....................................................................................................... 25 
I.D. Investigating a New Question: How Erroneous Convictions Differ from Near Misses .................. 29 
I.E. How Does the Criminal Justice System Identify Innocence Before Conviction? ............................ 31 

iv
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

I.E.1. Hypotheses: Comparing Erroneous Convictions and Near Misses .......................................... 32 
II. METHODS............................................................................................................................................. 38 
II.A. Case Criteria ................................................................................................................................... 38 
II.B. Identifying Qualifying Cases .......................................................................................................... 41 
II.C. Data Collection and Case Coding ................................................................................................... 45 
II.C.1. The Narrative Coding Document ............................................................................................. 45 
II.C.2. Sources of Case Facts .............................................................................................................. 46 
II.C.3. Coding Case Facts into SPSS .................................................................................................. 50 
II.D. Rating the Strength of Cases .......................................................................................................... 51 
II.D.1. Introduction to the Police Foundation Rating Scale ............................................................... 51 
II.D.2. Applying the Police Foundation Rating Scale to Project Cases ............................................. 52 
III. QUANTITATIVE ANALYSIS ............................................................................................................ 57 
III.A. Frequencies and Bivariate Analysis .............................................................................................. 57 
III.A.1. Statistically Significant Variables .......................................................................................... 57 
III.A.2. Variables That Do Not Distinguish the Cases ........................................................................ 60 
III.B. Logistic Regressions...................................................................................................................... 64 
III.B.1. Estimating the Models ............................................................................................................ 64 
III.B.2. Regression Results .................................................................................................................. 66 
III.B.3. Prediction/Forecasting ........................................................................................................... 70 
IV. QUALITATIVE ANALYSIS AND THE EXPERT PANEL .............................................................. 72 

v
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

IV.A. Convening the Expert Panel .......................................................................................................... 72 
IV.B. Qualitative Analysis ...................................................................................................................... 74 
IV.B.1. Predictor #1: Prior Convictions ............................................................................................. 74 
IV.B.2. Predictor #2: Intentional Misidentification ............................................................................ 76 
IV.B.3. Predictor #3: Forensic Errors ................................................................................................ 76 
IV.B.4. Predictor #4: Weak Prosecution Case .................................................................................... 78 
IV.B.5. Predictor #5: Weak Defense Case .......................................................................................... 81 
IV.B.6. The Role of the Judge.............................................................................................................. 83 
IV.B.7. How Factors Interact: Tunnel Vision ..................................................................................... 84 
V. DISCUSSION ........................................................................................................................................ 89 
V.A. Broader Interpretation .................................................................................................................... 90 
V.B. Recommendations for Reform ....................................................................................................... 94 
V.C. Study Limitations ......................................................................................................................... 101 
V.D. Possible Directions for Future Research ...................................................................................... 104 
VI. REFERENCES ................................................................................................................................... 107 
VII. TABLES AND FIGURES ................................................................................................................. 120 
Figure 1. Distribution of Cases by County Population ..................................................................... 120 
Table 1. Erroneous Convictions by State .......................................................................................... 121 
Table 2. Near Misses by State ........................................................................................................... 123 
Table 3. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Location Effects ............................................................. 125 

vi
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 4. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Nature of the Defendant ................................................. 126 
Table 5. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Nature of the Crime ........................................................ 127 
Table 6. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Quality of Work by Criminal Justice Officials .............. 129 
Table 7. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Nature of the Facts Available to the State ...................... 131 
Table 8. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Quality of Defense ......................................................... 135 
Table 9. Erroneous Convictions by Year of Crime ........................................................................... 136 
Table 10. Near Misses by Year of Crime .......................................................................................... 138 
Table 11. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Defendant, Significant Variables Controlled for Murder
and Sexual Assault ............................................................................................................................ 140 
Table 12. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Defendant, Significant Variables Controlled for Pre- and
Post-DNA Resolution ....................................................................................................................... 141 
Table 13. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Crime, Significant Variables Controlled for Murder and
Sexual Assault ................................................................................................................................... 142 
Table 14. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Crime, Significant Variables Controlled for Pre- and
Post-DNA Resolution ....................................................................................................................... 143 
Table 15. Bivariate Results: Quality of Work by Criminal Justice Officials, Significant Variables
Controlled for Murder and Sexual Assault ....................................................................................... 144 
Table 16. Bivariate Results: Quality of Work by Criminal Justice Officials, Significant Variables
Controlled for Pre- and Post-DNA Resolution ................................................................................. 145 
Table 17. Bivariate Results: Quality of Work by Criminal Justice Officials, Significant Variables
Controlled for Illinois ....................................................................................................................... 146 
Table 18. Bivariate Results: Location Effects, Significant Variables Controlled for Illinois ........... 147 

vii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 19. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Facts Available to the State, Significant Variables
Controlled for Murder and Sexual Assault ....................................................................................... 148 
Table 20. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Facts Available to the State, Significant Variables
Controlled for Pre- and Post-DNA Resolution ................................................................................. 151 
Table 21. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Quality of Defense: Significant Variables Controlled for
Murder and Sexual ............................................................................................................................ 154 
Table 22. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Quality of Defense: Significant Variables Controlled by
Pre-DNA and Post-DNA Testing ...................................................................................................... 155 
Table 23. Variables Included After Bivariate Controls .................................................................... 156 
Table 24. Logistic Regression Results: Nature of the Crime and Location Effect Model ................ 157 
Table 25. Logistic Regression Results: Nature of the Defendant Model .......................................... 158 
Table 26. Logistic Regression Results: Nature of the Facts Model .................................................. 159 
Table 27. Logistic Regression Results: Quality of the Criminal Justice System Model................... 160 
Table 28. Logistic Regression Results: Quality of Defense Model .................................................. 161 
Table 29. Factors that Influence the Likelihood of an Erroneous Conviction .................................. 162 
Figure 2. Sample Receiver-Operating Characteristic Curve, Model Includes Nature of the Crime and
Controls ............................................................................................................................................. 164 
Figure 3. Receiver-Operating Characteristic Curve for First Imputed Data Set, Final Model ......... 165 
Figure 4. Probability of an Erroneous Conviction ............................................................................ 166 
Table 30. Number of Complete Cases, Imputed Cases, and Models used for Imputation ................ 167 
Table 31. Summary Statistics for Non-imputed Data ....................................................................... 168 

viii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 32. Summary Statistics for Complete vs. Observed Data ....................................................... 169 
Table 33. Study Results by Hypothesis Proffered ............................................................................ 170 
Figure 5. Probability Density Plots of Observed, Imputed, and Complete Data for Age of Defendant
 .......................................................................................................................................................... 172 
Figure 6. Probability Density Plots of Observed, Imputed, and Complete Data for Logged Time from
Indictment to Arrest .......................................................................................................................... 173 
Figure 7. Divergent Processes of Innocence Cases ........................................................................... 174 
VIII. APPENDICES ................................................................................................................................. 175 
VIII.A. Case Coding Document ............................................................................................................ 175 
VIII.B. Coding Book (for Case Coding Document) ............................................................................. 209 
VIII.C. Email and Call Templates for Interviews ................................................................................. 235 
VIII.D. Informed Consent Form for Interviews .................................................................................... 245 
VIII.E. Statistical Coding Book ............................................................................................................ 248 
VIII.F. Sample Factors and Exemplars from the Modified Police Foundation Strength of Evidence
Rating Scale .......................................................................................................................................... 360 
VIII.G. Sample Case Narratives ........................................................................................................... 361 
VIII.G.1. Erroneous Conviction Exemplar: Highly Probative Prosecution and Weak Defense ....... 361 
VIII.G.2. Erroneous Conviction Exemplar: Probative Prosecution and No Defense ....................... 368 
VIII.G.3. Erroneous Conviction Exemplar: Weak Prosecution and Probative Defense .................. 374 
VIII.G.4. Near Miss Exemplar: Highly Probative Prosecution and Weak Defense ......................... 380 
VIII.G.5. Near Miss Exemplar: Probative Prosecution and Probative Defense .............................. 384 

ix
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

VIII.G.6. Near Miss Exemplar: Weak Prosecution and Highly Probative Defense ......................... 392 
VIII.H. Case Evaluation Sheet for Expert Panelists ............................................................................. 400 
VIII.I. Technical Appendix ................................................................................................................... 404 
VIII.I.A. Multiple Imputation ............................................................................................................ 404 
VIII.I.B. Receiver-Operating Characteristic Curves ........................................................................ 406 
VIII.I.C. Prediction/Forecasting ....................................................................................................... 409 

x
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

EXECUTIVE SUMMARY
Until 1988, when Gary Dotson became the first person to be exonerated through DNA
testing, exonerations were a rarity. At that time, very few could imagine that over the course of
the next 25 years, well over 300 individuals would be declared innocent and released from
prison. Like Dotson’s story, these cases have drawn upon emerging technologies—like DNA
analysis, cell phone triangulation, video facial recognition, and the debunking of previously
common forensic methods—to establish innocence. Yet, while the science involved in the
process of exoneration has advanced, research on erroneous convictions has remained relatively
static in its method. Erroneous conviction research, rather than adopting more empirical forms
of analysis, has continued to primarily rely upon the case study model. While this model can
illustrate what factors are shared by erroneous convictions, it cannot isolate causes. Here, we use
the social science method of case comparison to identify factors that distinguish erroneous
convictions from other cases in which factually innocent defendants are released or acquitted
post-indictment on the basis of their innocence.

Traditional “Causes” of Erroneous Convictions
In 1932, Edward Borchard’s Convicting the Innocent highlighted 65 cases of erroneously
convicted individuals. Borchard’s book was among the first to draw attention to erroneous
convictions in the United States, and it also established a methodological approach that would
become the norm in subsequent research. Later studies drew upon Borchard’s method of
dramatically narrating the cases, identifying factors shared between the cases, and discussing
what policy recommendations could remedy these “causes.” Researchers have used this method
to identify seven factors that appear most closely associated with erroneous convictions:

xi
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 Mistaken Eyewitness Identification: Nationally, over three-quarters of known erroneous
convictions (many of them in rape cases) involve eyewitness misidentifications (Garrett,
2011; Gross & Shaffer, 2012).

 False Confessions: Several studies of erroneous prosecutions conducted since 1987 have
shown that 14-25% of the cases reviewed involved false confessions (Leo, 2008).
According to the national Innocence Project, approximately two-thirds of the DNA
exonerations in homicide cases involved false confessions (White, 2003).

 Tunnel Vision: Tunnel vision is nearly impossible to quantify in terms of its prevalence
among erroneous convictions. However, previous qualitative and case study research
suggests that tunnel vision is common. Findley and Scott explain that when criminal
justice professionals “focus on a suspect, select and filter the evidence that will ‘build a
case’ for conviction, while ignoring the suppressing evidence that points away from
guilt,” they are at risk of “locking on” to the wrong suspect and inadvertently leading to
his continued prosecution and conviction (2006: 292).

 Perjured Informant Testimony: Frequently referred to as “snitch testimony,” the
Innocence Project reports that 15% of erroneous convictions that were overturned
through DNA evidence included perjured informant testimony (Innocence Project, n.d.).
In these situations, statements are solicited from an informant (who is often also involved

xii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

in the criminal justice system) through the promise of various incentives, which may not
be disclosed to the jury.

 Forensic Error: Forensic error involves both errors in testing, including the use of
unvalidated techniques, and testimony provided by lab technicians. The Innocence
Project indicates that forensic error was present in 50% of DNA exonerations nationwide
(Innocence Project, n.d.).

 Prosecutorial Error: Prosecutorial error, which includes Brady violations, can be caused
by inexperienced prosecutors who may not recognize what information should be shared
with the defense or by malicious misconduct. Gross and Shaffer (2012) found that 42%
of all exonerations involved misconduct or error by criminal justice officials (not limited
to just prosecutors).

 Inadequate Defense Representation: “Bad lawyering” is drawing increased attention as a
factor associated with erroneous conviction. A Columbia University study of capital
appeals found that ineffective defense lawyering was the biggest contributing factor to
the erroneous conviction of criminal defendants in capital cases over a 23-year period
(Liebman, Fagan, West, & Lloyd, 2000).
In addition to these factors, researchers have noted the potentially harmful effects on innocent
defendants of the media, racial bias, state jurisdiction, and their youth.

xiii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

But Are These Factors Causes?
Although research continues to show that erroneous convictions share these factors in
relatively large numbers, it is incorrect to call them “causes.” It is not clear that these same
factors occur at a significantly different frequency in other types of cases—for instance,
convictions where the criminal defendant is guilty or cases of innocent defendants who are
acquitted. Thus, a comparison group is needed to examine whether these factors are actually
predictors of erroneous conviction.
In our study, we created a comparison group of “near misses,” cases where a factually
innocent defendant was indicted but released before conviction on the basis of his innocence.
Our comparison was posed to answer two over-arching questions:
1. What factors explain why innocent suspects are erroneously convicted in certain
cases but acquitted or have their charges dismissed in other cases (“near misses”)?
2. What policy interventions will help the criminal justice system “get it right” and
acquit the innocent, thereby preventing future erroneous convictions?
To answer these questions, our study considered 23 hypotheses that drew upon previous research
on erroneous convictions (there is virtually no literature on near misses). The hypotheses
considered the traditional legal sources of error listed above, sociological factors such as race or
socioeconomic status, the structure of the investigative and adjudicative process, and actors and
actions within the criminal justice system. We then tested the hypotheses using a mixed method
approach.

xiv
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Selection of Cases
Our study included 460 cases from 1980-2012. Each case involved a factually innocent
defendant who was indicted by a state for a violent felony against a person and was subsequently
relieved of all legal responsibility for the crime. Two categories were created: “erroneous
convictions” for those who were exonerated after conviction (n=260), and “near misses” for
those who were acquitted or had charges dismissed before conviction on the basis of factual
innocence (n=200). The project employed a conservative definition of factual innocence that
clearly distinguishes factual innocence from innocence based on procedural error or other purely
legal criteria (so-called “legal innocence”). To establish factual innocence, each case had to
possess two separate components:
1. A judicial, executive or legislative acknowledgement that the individual did not commit
the crime for which he was erroneously indicted (including a statement of innocence by a
prosecutor, governor, judge, state compensation board, or a juror after an acquittal).
2. Evidence that would convince a reasonable person that the individual did not commit the
crime (such as post-conviction DNA testing, another individual was prosecuted and
convicted of the crime, or a new diagnosis of the victim’s condition).
Potential erroneous conviction and near miss cases were systematically identified using
the same, multi-faceted methods. Researchers examined previous academic scholarship,
databases, and media resources, and solicited cases from criminal justice professionals. Near
misses were more difficult to find than erroneous convictions because there has been little
previous research or advocacy associated with these cases. Therefore, researchers relied heavily
on newspaper archives and case recommendations from prosecutors, defense attorneys, and
scholars. For both sets of cases, researchers excluded those cases that did not meet the
xv
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

conservative definition of factual innocence; additionally, we attempted to include as many nonDNA cases as possible.

Quantitative Analysis
For each of the 460 qualifying cases, researchers collected data to answer 61 umbrella
questions on the project’s narrative coding document. The questions were designed to reflect
possible sources of erroneous conviction as suggested by prior research, as well as those factors
that might distinguish erroneous convictions from dismissals and acquittals based on innocence.
The questions involved three separate but non-exclusive categories of case facts: demographics,
procedural information, and evidential information. The questions were largely open-ended and
designed to elicit a narrative response. Researchers used newspaper articles, legal documents,
interviews with people involved with the cases, and other sources to answer the questions
completely. Over 600 statistical variables were created in SPSS to capture the 61 umbrella
questions in the narrative coding document.
Researchers also used an instrument developed by the Police Foundation to determine
the overall strength of the cases. The purpose was to allow us to distinguish between “easy”
cases, in which few people would have considered a defendant to be guilty, and “harder” cases,
where the facts of a case might have convinced many reasonable people to believe the defendant
was guilty even though he was innocent. The scale was originally designed by the Foundation as
a method of getting at “ground truth” (e.g., is the suspect really guilty?). As modified by the
project for our own purposes, the scale contains six categories of information or evidence; each
category in turn contains exemplars that have been given a rating on a 3-point scale (3 being
strong evidence, 1 being weak evidence). These exemplars serve as “anchors” for the researcher

xvi
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

when assessing how her own case facts should be rated. The case ratings were also added into
SPSS.
Our study evaluated which variables were related to case outcome using bivariate and
logistic regression analyses. In both types of analyses, we controlled for time period (postDNA), type of crime (murder or rape), and state. Using bivariate analysis, we found over 20
variables that were significantly correlated with case outcome. These statistically significant
variables were then further tested using logistic regressions.
Employing multiple imputation to recover lost data, we tested several logistic regression
models. The model with the best fit included the following ten statistically significant variables:
 State Death Penalty Culture (Executions per Population)
 Age of Defendant
 Criminal History of Defendant
 Strength of Prosecution’s Case
 Intentional Misidentification
 Forensic Evidence Error
 Prosecution Withheld Evidence
 Lying by Non-Eyewitness
 Strength of Defense
 Defendant Offered Family Witness
Our model can be used to accurately predict an erroneous conviction versus a near miss nearly
91% of the time. We expect that this model could be used to predict future cases across the
United States that are likely to be erroneous convictions with a high degree of accuracy.

xvii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Qualitative Methods
A vital component of the project was to supplement our quantitative results with a
qualitative context. We convened an expert panel to review 39 sample cases, drawn from our
database of erroneous convictions and near misses. The expert panel comprised twelve criminal
justice professionals including: two prosecutors, two retired judges, a defense attorney, a police
sergeant, a forensic scientist, and researchers on both police and prosecutor practices. Rather
than telling the experts what variables had been significant, we asked our panelists to identify
what they believed were the sources of error in the sample cases. Then, the panel discussed why
those factors either led to an erroneous conviction or a near miss, and what, if any, interventions
might have changed the case outcome. Noticably, the panel independently identifed as important
most of the factors that we found to be statistical predictors of case outcome, as well as
additional aspects such as tunnel vision that we were not able to isolate via quantitative analysis.
The panel discussion provided a valuable framework to understand how these factors interact
within the criminal justice system to affect the progress of a case.

Results & Discussion
Together, our logistic regression model and the expert panel identified the following
variables as significant factors that either harm or help the innocent defendant:

 State Punitiveness: Defendants in punitive states appear to be at an increased risk of
erroneous conviction once indicted. In a punitive legal culture, police and prosecutors
may be more interested in obtaining a conviction at all costs (leading to greater Brady
violations, etc.) and community pressure may encourage overly swift resolutions to cases

xviii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

involving serious crimes like rape and murder. Additionally, state punitiveness could
contribute to more state actors assuming the defendant’s guilt. This culture eventually
works against the defendant, as state agents overlook or under-value evidence that
contradicts the assumption of guilt.

 Strength of Prosecution’s Case, Brady Violations, and Lying Non-Eyewitness Evidence:
Unexpectedly, a weak prosecution’s case was more likely to lead to an erroneous
conviction than a near miss. Discussions with the panelists suggested that weak facts
may encourage prosecutors to engage in certain behaviors designed to bolster the case,
which our statistics show help predict an erroneous conviction. In several of our
erroneous convictions, a prosecutor, convinced of the defendant’s guilt despite a lack of
conclusive proof, failed to recognize and turn over exculpatory evidence or enlisted a
non-eyewitness (such as a snitch) to provide corroborating testimony. These types of
actions compound, rather than rectify, previous errors or misconduct in the case, resulting
in an escalation of commitment. Eventually, despite the weak evidence, the players
involved become so committed to proving the defendant’s guilt that evidence illustrating
the contrary is ignored or discounted.

 Forensic Error: Error in forensic evidence presented by the prosecution was correlated
with an increased likelihood of erroneous conviction. This error most often occurred in
testimony or interpretation of evidence, rather than in the actual scientific testing. Errors
in forensic testimony include: neglecting to provide the jury with key information such as
the victim’s blood type when it would mask the perpetrator’s; overstating the inculpatory

xix
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

nature of the evidence by providing inaccurate or non-existent statistics; and misstating
the certainty of the results when the forensic technique, such as bite mark, scent, or fiber
analysis, does not allow for it.

 Age and Prior History of Defendant: Both a defendant’s age and prior record may harm
an innocent defendant. In our dataset, young defendants were at an increased likelihood
of conviction. A younger defendant will often not have the sophistication or knowledge
to aid in his defense and may be slow to realize the gravity of the situation; he may also
have a harder time proving a credible alibi. Panelists also discussed how a defendant’s
prior criminal history can bias police and prosecutors into prematurely narrowing the
focus on the defendant and ignoring potentially exculpatory evidence.

 Intentional Misidentification by Eyewitness: Although the overall frequency of
misidentifications was not significantly different between the erroneous convictions and
near misses, when broken down by intentionally false versus honestly mistaken
identifications a significant difference did emerge. Malicious implication of a defendant
led to a decreased likelihood of erroneous conviction; or, put another way, an honest,
inadvertent mistake increased the likelihood of erroneous conviction. Prosecutors on our
panel discussed how a thorough vetting of complaining witnesses can more easily
identify inconsistencies in initial statements and red flags when the witness is lying. A
truly mistaken identification is harder to uncover even with proper investigation.

xx
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 Strength of Defense: As expected, a stronger defense tended to lead to a dismissal or
acquittal of an innocent defendant. Though bad lawyering has received much attention in
the discussion of erroneous conviction, our panel was struck by the presence of
exceptionally good lawyering among the near misses. These defense attorneys often did
months of leg-work, hired experts, and most importantly, persisted in proving the
defendant’s innocence rather than immediately working on obtaining a plea. Notably, the
type of the defense attorney was not significant in either the quantitative or qualitative
analysis. Poor representation—regardless of whether it was a private attorney or public
defender—was what really determined case outcome.

 Tunnel Vision and System Failure: Discussions with the panel highlighted a particularly
important factor not explored in the quantitative analysis—namely, tunnel vision or
escalation of commitment. According to the panelists, tunnel visions helps explain how
one error often leads to additional errors in an erroneous conviction. It contributes and
facilitates system breakdown because it dismantles the rigorous testing of evidence that
makes the investigative and adversarial processes function effectively. Ultimately, we
concluded that what separates erroneous convictions from near misses is not just a list of
individual factors, but more importantly the process by which initial errors remain
undetected or uncorrected in the erroneous convictions and lead to system failure.
Indeed, if there is but one conclusion from our research it is that, overall, the erroneously
convicted are truly cases of systemic failure.
Interestingly, our analysis did not identify a number of the traditional sources of
erroneous convictions as significant predictors. These included the race of the defendant, snitch
xxi
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

testimony, false confessions, police error, and many eyewitness testimony variables (such as
interracial identification, certainty of the witness, and type of identification procedure). While
these variables do not determine case outcome, some of them, like false confessions, police error,
and an African American defendant, appear in a relatively large proportion of both the erroneous
conviction and near misses cases. We suggest that rather than being predictors of erroneous
conviction, such factors may explain why innocent individuals are originally brought into the
criminal justice system.
Ultimately, in our study the impact of traditional “causes” of erroneous convictions on
case outcome is split. Some—such as prosecutorial misconduct, strong defense, forensic error,
and lying by a non-eyewitness—do determine case outcome. However, even among these
expected findings, our results suggest the way these factors impact case outcome may be
different than accepted knowledge. For example, forensic error contributes not so much in terms
of error in testing, but rather by error in testimony. As a result, previous policy
recommendations that have focused on improving the quality of forensic laboratory procedures
should be revisited to emphasize quality control at the interpretation and testimony stages.
Furthermore, our research highlights that the interactions of factors, as much as the individual
factors themselves, are responsible for the system breakdowns that result in erroneous
convictions. Thus, reform efforts should increasingly focus on more comprehensive approaches
to combat systemic problems such as tunnel vision.
Future research should continue to use a social science approach as an effective method
to study miscarriages of justice. We suggest exploring alternative comparison groups to
erroneous convictions, such as defendants who are convicted and not exonerated. Finally, we

xxii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

encourage more research on near misses to better learn how the criminal justice system can “get
it right” when confronted with an innocent defendant.

xxiii
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

I. INTRODUCTION
I.A. Background
In the last two decades, hundreds of people erroneously convicted of rape and murder
have been exonerated and released from prison (Gross & Shaffer, 2012). Many of these
exonerations occurred after post-conviction DNA testing established the innocence of the
defendants (Innocence Project, n.d.). In some of these cases—such as the Central Park jogger
case in New York City and the Beatrice Six case in Nebraska—DNA evidence established the
innocence of multiple defendants who had been erroneously prosecuted, convicted, and
incarcerated years earlier (Garrett, 2011). In other cases, innocent individuals were released
from prison after serving many years on death row (Cohen, 2003). To date, more than 290
individuals have been exonerated by post-conviction DNA testing (Innocence Project, n.d.), and
there appears to be no let up in the steady stream of prisoners whose innocence is established by
increasingly sophisticated DNA testing (Garrett, 2011). An increasing number of the wrongly
convicted also have established their innocence through non-DNA means of exoneration in the
last twenty years (Gross & Shaffer, 2012).
The exoneration of hundreds of erroneously convicted but factually innocent individuals
has challenged some of our most fundamental assumptions about the American criminal justice
and legal systems. Prior to 1989, the first year that post-conviction DNA testing was used to
establish innocence (Scheck, Neufeld & Dwyer, 2000), virtually all observers assumed that the
innocent were rarely convicted (Leo, 2005), if at all, especially in capital cases (Bedau &
Radelet, 1987). Since 1989, however, there has been a growing recognition in popular culture
and among criminal justice professionals that erroneous convictions occur regularly in the

1
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

American criminal justice system (Findley, 2011). In American society, the arrival of DNA
testing and the jump in factually indisputable exonerations has put the problem of erroneous
conviction on the national agenda and led to a drop in public confidence about the criminal
justice system (Gould, 2007; Baumgartner, De Boef, & Boydstun, 2008). As a result of these
and other developments, there has been renewed and concerted attention to the causes and
consequences of erroneous conviction.
The DNA exonerations have given rise to an “innocence movement” in American law
(Zalman, 2011), seen by some as “the civil rights movement of the twenty-first century”
(Medwed, 2008). Although this is likely an overstatement, across the United States at various
law schools there are now more than 70 non-profit innocence projects whose purpose is to
investigate and litigate post-conviction claims of innocence as well as propose reforms
(Innocence Network, n.d.). Additionally, many states have passed legislation to make
exoneration easier and prevent erroneous convictions. Six states have created innocence
commissions (Gould, 2007), and more than 40 state legislatures have passed statutes to facilitate
inmate access to biological evidence for post-conviction DNA testing. Other states have
implemented legislation to address certain sources of erroneous conviction (Medwed, 2008).
Even the United States Congress has addressed erroneous convictions by passing the 2004
Innocence Protection Act, which provides funding for state post-conviction DNA testing and
raises the annual compensation for exonerated federal prisoners (Innocence Protection Act,
2004).
Although the precise rate of erroneous convictions remains unknown and unknowable
(Simon, 2012), there is a growing awareness in the legal system that erroneous convictions occur
more frequently than almost anyone had previously thought (Findley, 2011). Former Supreme

2
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Court Justice David Souter reflected this sentiment when he wrote that erroneous convictions (in
capital cases) happen “in numbers never imagined before the development of DNA tests”
(Kansas v. Marsh, 2006: 2544). After analyzing hundreds of post-1989 exonerations, Gross
Jacoby, Matheson, Montgomery, & Patil (2005: 551) made this point even more forcefully: “any
plausible guess at the total number of miscarriages of justice in America in the last fifteen years
must be in the thousands, perhaps tens of thousands.” Indeed, a recent report by the Urban
Institute found erroneous convictions in five percent of homicide and sexual assault cases in
Virginia during 1973-1987 (Roman, Walsh, Lachman, & Jahner, 2012).
The exoneration of hundreds of innocent prisoners in the last two decades also has had an
effect on academic scholarship, generating a virtual explosion of research on wrongful
convictions (Leo, 2005). Strikingly, most of this scholarship has occurred in law reviews, not in
peer reviewed social science or criminology journals. In the last decade, law professors and
legal scholars have written extensively about the legal causes and consequences of wrongful
conviction, as well as about legal and policy reforms designed to reduce their occurrence (Leo &
Gould, 2009). Beyond the large quantity of legal scholarship, there also has been a sense that a
new paradigm may be emerging, one that treats accuracy and reliability within the legal system
as on par with or even more important than traditional concerns about procedural due process
(Thomas, 2008). Daniel Medwed has coined the term “innocentrism” to connote the emerging
centrality of innocence-based arguments and scholarship in criminal law and procedure
(Medwed, 2008). Yet for all the attention that academe has given to wrongful convictions, there
has been relatively little dialogue on this topic between criminal law scholars and lawyers on the
one hand and criminologists or social scientists on the other (Leo & Gould, 2009).

3
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

The study of wrongful convictions actually has a long history in American scholarship
(Leo, 2005). For almost a century, writers have documented erroneous convictions of the
innocent and described their causes and consequences. Yale Law School professor Edward
Borchard’s 1932 book, Convicting the Innocent, is generally regarded as the breakout work on
the subject. Borchard documented 65 cases in which innocent defendants were wrongfully
convicted; in addition to identifying the legal causes underlying these convictions, has also
proposed reforms to remedy the problem (Borchard, 1932).
Convicting the Innocent was significant because it shifted the debate away from whether
factually innocent individuals were erroneously convicted in the American criminal justice
system to why these wrongful convictions occurred and how they could be prevented. The book
became the template for future wrongful conviction studies and outlined a simple formula:
identify wrongful conviction cases, describe their legal causes, and propose reforms to prevent
future miscarriages. Other writers such as Gardner (1952), Frank and Frank (1957) and Radin
(1964) would continue in this tradition for many decades to come, but the exoneration narratives
described in these books failed to persuade others that wrongful convictions represented a
systemic problem in the criminal justice system as opposed to a few anomalous, if deeply
troubling, travesties of justice. For decades, the problem of wrongful conviction generated very
little interest among criminal justice officials, policy-makers, or the public.
In 1987, Hugo Bedau and Michael Radelet’s article, “Miscarriages of Justice in
Potentially Capital Cases,” ushered in a new era and form of wrongful conviction research.
Bedau and Radelet documented and systematically analyzed 350 cases of wrongful conviction—
based mostly, but not entirely, on official declarations of innocence—in potentially capital cases
from 1900 to 1985, 23 of which they argued had led to wrongful executions (Bedau & Radelet,

4
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1987). By uncovering so many cases of erroneous conviction—even in capital cases—Bedau
and Radelet challenged the traditional assumptions about the infallibility of human judgments in
the criminal justice system. Indeed, they illustrated that mistakes happened, even in the most
serious cases. Bedau and Radelet also analyzed patterns in the sources of errors, sources of
discovery of the errors, and sources of exoneration across these cases.
Bedau and Radelet triggered intense debate about the risk of executing the innocent.
Following their lead, more scholars and journalists began to write about the problem of wrongful
convictions in the late 1980s and early 1990s (Yant, 1991; Huff, Rattner, & Sagarin, 1996;
Connery, 1996). Bedau and Radelet continued to collect, analyze, and publish research on
wrongful convictions in capital cases (Bedau & Radelet, 2004), and others reanalyzed their data
(Gross, 1996, 1998; Acker, Brewer, & Cunningham, 2001). But, to some extent, Bedau and
Radelet’s message became mired in disagreements about whether a small number of individuals
in their study were actually guilty and, more generally, about whether their scholarship was just
another ideological move in the contentious debate about the desirability of the death penalty in
America (Markman & Cassell, 1988; Bedau & Radelet, 1988).
In the late 1980s, the renewed interest in wrongful convictions was catapulted forward by
the introduction of DNA testing in criminal cases (Gould, 2007). In 1989, DNA was first used to
exonerate an innocent prisoner, Gary Dotson, who had been wrongfully incarcerated for ten
years in Illinois for a rape he did not commit (Gross et al., 2005). In the twenty plus years since
Dotson’s exoneration, post-conviction DNA testing has led to the release of more than 290
wrongly convicted individuals (Innocence Project, n.d.) and contributed to dramatically changing
official and popular attitudes about the criminal justice system (Baumgartner et al., 2008).
Baumgartner et al. (2008) suggest these changing attitudes could be influenced by noticeable

5
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

changes in the tone of press coverage of the death penalty. Indeed, Baumgartner et al. found that
as more exonerations occurred, the New York Times’ tone towards the death penalty became
more negative. Relatedly, many of the individual DNA exonerations have garnered tremendous
coverage in print and electronic media (Warden, 2003; Garrett, 2011), and the DNA cases
collectively have provided an unprecedented opportunity to better understand the nature and
consequences of factual error in the American criminal justice system (Garrett, 2011). These
DNA exonerations, and the substantial media attention they have received, have inspired
extensive legal scholarship to the point that law reviews are now full of articles on the various
legal causes and problems of wrongful conviction cases as well as proposed reforms (Leo &
Gould, 2009).
While erroneous conviction research has increased, the methods and approach have not
changed much. Most scholarship still uses the traditional narrative or “familiar plot”—one that
dates back to Borchard’s template—but with new and updated cases (Leo, 2005). Perhaps no
book illustrates the modern version of this story better than Barry Scheck, Peter Neufeld, and Jim
Dwyer’s (2000) Actual Innocence: Five Days to Execution and Other Dispatches From the
Wrongly Convicted, which may be the signature document of the modern innocence movement.
Written by two well-known criminal defense lawyers, one of whom is also a law professor, and a
journalist, Actual Innocence is the rare book that has been both an influential work of legal
scholarship and a popular best-seller (Lain, 2007). The only other post-DNA era writing on
wrongful conviction to reach a mass audience was John Grisham’s (2006) non-fiction book, The
Innocent Man. Nevertheless, as we will discuss below, the implications of these books – from
Borchard (1932) to Scheck et al. (2000) and Grisham (2006) – are limited by the narrative
method (Leo & Gould, 2009).

6
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

I. B. What Do We Know Now? Factors that Correlate with Erroneous Conviction
The research on erroneous conviction has generally identified seven primary sources of
error: (1) mistaken eyewitness identification; (2) false confessions; (3) tunnel vision; (4) perjured
informant testimony; (5) forensic error; (6) prosecutorial error; and (7) inadequate defense
representation. Apart from these principal sources, the wrongful conviction research literature
also discusses the potential role of race effects, media effects, and the failure of post-conviction
remedies (Gould & Leo, 2010).
I.B.1 Mistaken Eyewitness Identification
Nationally, over three-quarters of known erroneous convictions (many of them in rape
cases) are, in part, the result of mistaken eyewitness identifications (Garrett, 2011; Gross &
Shaffer, 2012). Eyewitness misidentification is caused by natural psychological errors in human
judgment. As Gary Wells and colleagues have noted, stress alters people’s perception of an
event. When confronted with a gun or other weapon during a violent crime, for example, the
victim may focus so heavily on the firearm that he or she cannot take in and remember well the
details of the perpetrator (Wells & Murray, 1983). This problem is more pronounced when the
victim and perpetrator are of different races (Meissner & Brigham, 2001). Victims may believe
that they recall the events accurately—the crime ostensibly “stenciled into their minds”—but
research indicates that there is little relationship between an eyewitness’s certainty of her
identification and the accuracy of that report (Wells & Murray, 1983).
Eyewitness identifications also can be influenced by the suggestiveness of the
identification process, which “leads eyewitnesses to distort their reports of the witnessing
experience across a broad array of questions” (Wells & Bradfield, 1998: 367). In practice,
suggestion can enter the identification process in two ways. First, law enforcement officers or
7
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

other observers can confirm a witness’s identification, whether at the time of the identification
procedure or at any point before in-court identification (Rosenthal & Rubin, 1978; Wells &
Quinlivan, 2009). This can be as subtle as an officer praising the witness for a “good job” in her
identification or as overt as a detective thanking the witness “for confirming our suspicion.” The
problem with such suggestions is that they can give witnesses false confidence in their
identifications, even if the witnesses are mistaken. Moreover, witnesses too rarely recognize that
a reinforcing comment inflated their confidence (Wells & Murray, 1983).
Second, law enforcement officers may employ suggestive identification procedures that
make the suspect stand out from others. For example, in the case of Marvin Anderson,
Anderson’s photograph appeared in color while the other photographs in the array were blackand-white (Gould, 2007). A further example is lineups, in which problems have arisen when the
suspect is the only person presented of a particular height, hair color, or complexion among a
group of six or more. These frailties may lead witnesses to make “relative judgments,” subtly
encouraging them to select the individual in an identification procedure who looks most like the
offender, rather than employing independent judgment to ensure that the individual identified is
the actual perpetrator (Wells, 2008). Often, someone in a lineup or photo array looks more like
the actual offender than the others do, and witnesses, in turn, may be tempted to identify that
person (Wells, Penrod, Malpass, Fulero, & Brimacombe, 1998). Additionally, any initial
mistaken identification may reinforce subsequent reports, because eyewitnesses may confuse or
replace their memory of the true perpetrator with the image of the person who looked most like
the offender in the identification procedure (Gould, 2007).
Given documented problems such as these, U.S. Attorney General Janet Reno
commissioned a group of criminal justice professionals in the late 1990s to address and

8
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

recommend guidelines for police identification procedures. Published by the National Institute
of Justice in 1999, these guidelines provide the basis for best practices in law enforcement
agencies around the country (Wells & Olson, 2003). The state of New Jersey, for example, has
adopted the recommendations. Among these best practices, researchers recommend that
witnesses be shown photographs or individuals in a lineup sequentially—that is, one at a time—
rather than simultaneously as a group (National Institute of Justice, 1999). Researchers also
recommend that witnesses be asked to determine, upon looking at each photograph or individual,
whether the witness recognizes the perpetrator. In an analysis of 25 studies comparing
simultaneous and sequential identification procedures, scholars have estimated that sequential
procedures can reduce the chances of a mistaken identification by nearly one-half (Steblay,
Dysart, Fulero, & Lindsay, 2001). Perhaps most importantly, identification procedures must be
administered “double-blind” so that neither the eyewitness nor the person administering the
lineup knows the identity of the prime suspect and thus cannot guess about or hint at the
correctness of the identification. In this way, suggestion and feedback effects can be minimized.
I.B.2 False Confessions
It is difficult for the public to understand why someone would confess to a crime that the
individual did not commit (Leo & Liu, 2009; Blandon-Gitlin, Sperry, & Leo, 2011), but research
not only indicates that false confessions occur but also explains how they happen (Leo, 2008;
Kassin et al., 2010). Several studies of erroneous prosecutions conducted since 1987 have
shown that anywhere from 14 percent to 25 percent of the cases reviewed involved false
confessions (Leo, 2008). According to the national Innocence Project, approximately two-thirds
of the DNA exonerations in homicide cases involved false confessions (White, 2003). This is
consistent with Warden’s finding that approximately 60 percent of erroneous homicide
9
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

convictions in Illinois since 1970 involved false confessions. Moreover, false confessions when
introduced into evidence at trial usually lead to the conviction of the innocent (Drizin & Leo,
2004).
There is no one cause, logic, or type of false confession. Rather, police-induced false
confessions are the product of a multiple step process of influence, persuasion, and compliance.
They usually involve psychological coercion (Ofshe & Leo, 1997). Under certain conditions of
interrogation, police are more likely to elicit false confessions, and certain types of individuals
are more vulnerable to interrogation pressure and, thus, are more easily manipulated into giving
false confessions. In order to understand why innocent suspects sometimes make false
confessions, first we must look at the process through which police investigators identify
criminal suspects and how police interrogation works as a psychological process, both in the preadmission and post-admission stages of interrogation.
Three errors occur in sequence when police elicit a false confession that leads to a
wrongful conviction. The first error occurs when detectives mistakenly classify an innocent
person as guilty. As Davis and Leo point out, “once specific suspects are targeted, police
interviews and interrogations are thereafter guided by the presumption of guilt” (2006: 123).
Whether to interrogate is arguably the most critical decision point in the investigative process.
Police only elicit false confessions if they erroneously interrogate innocent people. If all the
suspects the police interrogated were, in fact, guilty, they would never elicit false confessions
from the innocent. Misclassifying innocent suspects is thus both the first and the most
consequential error police interrogators make.
Although many cognitive errors lead police to mistakenly classify an innocent person as a
guilty suspect, perhaps the most common errors are the product of their investigative training.

10
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Police officers in the United States are erroneously taught that they can learn to become human
lie detectors, able to distinguish truth from deception at extraordinarily high rates of accuracy
(Leo, 2008). For example, detectives are taught that the following behaviors are symptomatic of
deceptive, and thus guilty, suspects: averting one’s gaze, slouching, shifting body posture,
touching one’s nose, adjusting or cleaning one’s glasses, chewing one’s fingernails, and stroking
the back of one’s head. Suspects who are guarded, uncooperative, and offer broad denials and
qualified responses are also believed to be lying and thus guilty. However, across a variety of
contexts, social science studies have repeatedly shown that individuals are highly prone to error
in their judgments about whether an individual is lying or telling the truth and, thus, are poor
human lie detectors. Studies show that most people accurately make these types of judgments at
rates no better than the flip of a coin (Leo, 2009). Moreover, studies have suggested that police
interrogators themselves cannot accurately distinguish between truthful and false denials of guilt
at levels greater than chance but, instead, routinely make erroneous judgments when trying to
separate the innocent from the guilty (Hartwig, Granhag, Strömwall, & Vrij, 2004; Kassin &
Fong, 1999).
Once detectives misclassify an innocent person as a guilty suspect, the next step is to
subject him to an accusatorial interrogation. Obtaining a confession becomes especially
important when there is little or no other evidence against the suspect—especially in high profile
cases in which police detectives are under great pressure to solve the crime (Gross, 1996)—and
typically no credible evidence exists against an innocent suspect who police erroneously believe
is guilty. Perhaps not surprisingly, the vast majority of documented false confession cases occur
in homicides and high profile cases (Drizin & Leo, 2004; Gross et al., 2005).

11
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

The primary cause of police-induced false confessions is the use of psychologically coercive
police interrogation methods (Leo, 2008). These include methods that were once identified with
the old “third degree,” such as deprivation (of food, sleep, water, or access to bathroom facilities,
for example), incommunicado interrogation, and extreme induced exhaustion and fatigue. Since
the 1940s, however, these techniques have become rare in domestic police interrogations.
Instead, when today’s police interrogators employ psychologically coercive techniques, they
usually consist of implicit or explicit promises of leniency and implicit or explicit threats of
harsher treatment in combination with other interrogation techniques such as accusation,
repetition, attacks on denials, and false evidence ploys (Ofshe & Leo, 1997; Leo, 2008).
Although psychological coercion is the primary cause of police-induced false
confessions, individuals differ in their ability to withstand interrogation pressure and, therefore,
in their vulnerability to giving false confessions (Gudjonsson, 2003). Individuals who are highly
suggestible or compliant are more likely to confess falsely. So, too, are the developmentally
disabled, cognitively impaired, juveniles, and the mentally ill—all of whom tend to be unusually
suggestible and compliant. The developmentally disabled are more likely to confess falsely for a
variety of reasons (Kassin et al., 2010). Youth is also a significant risk factor for police-induced
false confessions (Kassin et al., 2010). Finally, people with mental illness are disproportionately
likely to falsely confess (Redlich, 2004), especially in response to accusatorial police pressure
(Salas, 2004).
The use of psychologically coercive police methods (and how they interact with an
individual’s personality) usually explains how and why interrogation succeeds in moving an
innocent suspect from denial to admission. But a confession consists not only of an “I did it”
statement, but also of a subsequent narrative—what researchers have referred to as the post-

12
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

admission narrative (Leo & Ofshe, 1998)—that contextualizes and attempts to explain the “I did
it” statement, transforming the admission into a confession. A detailed post-admission narrative
is what makes the story appear to be a compelling account of the suspect’s guilt. The content
and structure of a suspect’s post-admission narrative goes a long way toward explaining why
confessions are treated as such powerful evidence of guilt and sometimes lead to the erroneous
conviction of the innocent (Leo, 2008).
Police detectives use the post-admission phase of interrogation to influence, shape, and
sometimes even script the suspect’s narrative. The detective’s ultimate objective is to elicit a
persuasive account of what happened that successfully incriminates the suspect and leads to his
conviction. For example, in false confession cases interrogators have sometimes invented,
suggested, or elicited an account of the suspect’s motivation. They often use scenario-based
inducements as a method of attributing a minimizing motive to the suspect—which the suspect
agrees to and then repeats back, even if it is completely inaccurate, because he comes to believe
that it will reduce his culpability. Police interrogators also will encourage suspects to attribute
their decision to confess to an act of conscience, to express remorse about committing the crime,
and to provide vivid details of the crime scene that appear to corroborate the suspect’s guilty
knowledge and thus confirm his culpability. In addition, interrogators will try to make the
admission appear voluntarily given, portraying the suspect as the agent of his own confession
and themselves merely as its passive recipients (Leo, 2008).
Police detectives help create false confessions in the post-admission narrative phase of
interrogation by pressuring the suspect to accept a particular account and suggesting crime facts
to him, thereby contaminating the suspect’s post-admission narrative. Unless the suspect has
learned the crime scene facts from the media, community gossip, or overheard conversations, an

13
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

innocent person will not know either the mundane or the dramatic details of the crime (Leo &
Ofshe, 1998). Absent such contamination, the innocent suspect’s post-admission narrative
should therefore be replete with errors when responding to questions for which the answers
cannot easily be guessed by chance. Unless, of course, the answers are implied, suggested, or
explicitly provided to the suspect—which, unfortunately, does occur in many false confession
cases (Leo, Drizin, Neufeld, Hall, & Vatner, 2006). When an interrogation is recorded, it may be
possible to trace, step by step, how and when the interrogator implied or suggested the correct
answers for the suspect to incorporate into his post-admission narrative. However, when the
interrogation is not recorded—and the interrogations preceding virtually all of the documented
false confession cases have not been recorded—then there may be no objective way to prove the
interrogator contaminated the suspect’s post-admission narrative. The contamination of the
suspect’s post-admission narrative is thus the third mistake in the trilogy of police errors that,
cumulatively, lead to the elicitation and construction of a suspect’s false confession (Leo &
Drizin, 2010).
Although police training is important in identifying and thus avoiding an erroneous
confession, research indicates that electronically recording interrogations can minimize the
likelihood that a false confession will lead to an erroneous conviction (Leo, 2008). Not only are
law enforcement officers more careful in interrogating suspects when they know a jury may view
the proceedings—abstaining from threats, punishment, or undue coaching—jurors also can
evaluate the circumstances of the interrogation to determine the accuracy of the witness’
statements (Sullivan, 2005). In the case of Earl Washington, Jr., for example, a videotape would
have shown officers holding up a key piece of evidence for Washington to describe rather than
creating the impression at trial that Washington had freely described a secret piece of evidence

14
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

known only to the perpetrator. For that matter, electronic recording presents advantages for law
enforcement officers who conduct proper interrogations. Videotaped evidence can be quite
compelling for jurors, and there is reason to believe that suspects are more likely to plead guilty
to a crime when a properly administered interrogation shows them confessing to the crime. Such
evidence also may stave off meritless civil suits when judges and jurors can see for themselves
how officers behaved in the interrogation room. It is no wonder that surveys of officers using
videotape find that many “enthusiastically support this practice” (Sullivan, 2005: 1128).
I.B.3. Tunnel Vision
Like any of us, police officers and prosecutors are susceptible to tunnel vision. That is,
the more law enforcement practitioners become convinced of a conclusion—in this case, a
suspect’s guilt—the less likely they are to consider alternative scenarios that conflict with this
conclusion. As Findley and Scott (2006: 292) explain more comprehensively, when criminal
justice professionals “focus on a suspect, select and filter the evidence that will ‘build a case’ for
conviction, while ignoring the suppressing evidence that points away from guilt,” they are at risk
of “locking on” to the wrong suspect and inadvertently leading to his continued prosecution and
conviction.
Tunnel vision can occur at any point in the criminal justice process (Raeder, 2003). An
officer may be so convinced of an eyewitness’s identification that he ignores other case facts that
point away from the suspect’s guilt; a forensic scientist may conduct a hair comparison and see
such a close match between that of the perpetrator and a suspect that he overlooks fingerprint
analysis that isn’t as compelling; a prosecutor may be so satisfied with a suspect’s confession
that he discounts forensic evidence that inculpates others; or a defense lawyer may consider the
prosecution’s case so airtight that he doesn’t bother to look deeper into the government’s files.
15
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Any of these possibilities may explain why innocent individuals are named as suspects and
prosecuted all the way to a conviction. These are not just theoretical possibilities; the many case
studies of wrongful convictions show that errors attributable to tunnel vision are real and have
grievous consequences (Gould, 2007).
I.B.4. Perjured Informant Testimony
A number of erroneous convictions have turned on the testimony of police informants
who themselves lied for personal gain. As scholars note, informants are often rewarded without
regard to the accuracy and reliability of their information (Zimmerman, 2001), with as many as
one-fifth of erroneous conviction cases based on snitches that lied (Natapoff, 2006). A classic
case is that of Jeffrey Cox in Virginia. Cox’s conviction for abduction and murder was made
largely on the testimony of two witnesses, whose prior felony convictions and pending charges
were not disclosed to the defense. Each of these facts would have undermined the credibility of
the witnesses, but instead of sharing this information with the defense, the prosecution vouched
for the veracity of both witnesses in its closing argument (Gould, 2007). As a federal appellate
judge has said of informant testimony, the government relies too heavily on witnesses who are
“rewarded criminals,” which compromises both the accuracy and the legitimacy of the criminal
justice system. “Because the government decides whether and when to use such witnesses, and
what, if anything, to give them for their service, the government . . . can either contribute to or
eliminate the problem,” the judge noted (Trott, 1996: 1382).
I.B.5. Forensic Error
Given the rise and wide acceptance of DNA testing, it is possible to forget that, for
decades, law enforcement had to rely on much less accurate forensic methods. Perhaps the most
famous practice is fingerprinting, a method so common that applicants for many sensitive jobs
16
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

have had to submit to a series of fingerprints. But evidence is now mounting about the problems
of fingerprinting analysis (Cole, 2001), which include a lack of validity testing and an absence of
validated standards for declaring a match (Mnookin, 2008). In fact, a Maryland trial judge has
ruled that latent fingerprint identification is not sufficiently reliable to be admissible into
evidence (National Research Council, 2009). The substance behind this conclusion was
bolstered by a recent National Research Council report on forensic evidence that echoed Haber
and Haber’s (2008) conclusion that “we have reviewed the available scientific evidence of the
validity of the ACE-V method [of latent fingerprint identification] and found none” (National
Research Council, 2009: 136-145).
Fingerprint analysis is hardly the most questionable forensic method employed. More
troubling is hair comparison analysis, in which hairs found at a crime scene are compared under
a microscope to those of a possible suspect. Although hair comparison analysis has passed the
Frye (1923) and Daubert (1993) standards in many courts and has been admitted into evidence,
more recent research raises considerable doubts about its accuracy (Gould & Leo, 2010). For
example, the Law Enforcement Assistance Administration Laboratory Proficiency Testing
Program, involving over 235 crime laboratories throughout the United States, found hair
comparison analysis to be the weakest of all forensic laboratory techniques tested, with error
rates as high as 67 percent on individual samples and the majority of laboratories reaching
incorrect results on four out of five hair samples analyzed. Another study found that hair
comparison error rates dropped from 30 percent to four percent when common hair comparison
methods, which compare a questioned hair to the hair samples of a suspect, were changed to a
“lineup” method, in which examiners compare a hair sample from the crime scene to samples
from five potential suspects (Gould, 2007).

17
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Another potentially problematic test has been serology analysis, which seeks to establish
the probability that a perpetrator and suspect share the same blood type. By contrast to DNA
testing, serology analysis does not specifically identify suspects, but jurors may not appreciate
this fact, hearing testimony of similar blood types as proof of identity “with as much
definitiveness as science can muster” (Moenssens, 1993: 13). Of course, that is no longer the
case.
DNA testing has helped to uncover the frailties of forensic methods used previously.
This said, DNA is not a panacea. There is always the small probability that the results will be
inaccurate, but more importantly, few crime scenes have sufficient, specific biological evidence
for DNA analysis. A robber may never touch a victim nor shed hairs or other biological markers
in a spot specific to himself (Leo & Gould, 2010). Yet the most significant hurdle, for many
police departments, is simply the cost of DNA testing (Pratt, Gaffney, Lovrich, & Johnson,
2006). Even large, well-funded departments with their own forensic lab, such as the New York
Police Department, cited cost as one reason why DNA testing is used in just seven percent of
homicide investigations (Schroeder & White, 2009). The prohibitive cost of DNA testing can
make it a “tool of last resort” that departments turn to only after other pieces of evidence have
been examined (Schroeder & White, 2009: 337). As a result, law enforcement often relies on
other evidence, including different forms of forensic analysis that carry with them greater risks
of inaccuracy.
Apart from the inherent weaknesses of various forms of forensic evidence, there have
been several shocking examples of improper, indeed shoddy, laboratory practices and forensic
testimony that have led to the conviction of innocent defendants (Garrett & Neufeld, 2009). The
problem is so serious that the National Research Council concluded in 2009 that “the forensic

18
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

science system [in the United States is] fragmented and the quality of practice uneven…These
shortcomings pose a threat to the quality and credibility of forensic science practice and its
service to the justice system” (National Research Council, 2009: 140).

I.B.6. Prosecutorial Error
For the most part, American prosecutors conduct themselves ethically, seeking to mete
out justice even if it means dismissing charges against a defendant whose criminality they
suspect but cannot establish. Still, prosecutors may engage in overly suggestive witness
coaching (Medwed, 2012), offer inappropriate and incendiary closing arguments (Medwed,
2012), or fail to disclose critical evidence to the defense, all of which may raise the prospect of
an erroneous conviction. In research on erroneous convictions, the most commonly established
transgression is the prosecution’s failure to turn over exculpatory evidence. Sometimes, police
officers do not provide prosecutors with this evidence, or prosecutors may be unaware that they
have such information in their files. In other cases, though, the misdeeds are intentional.
Consider the case of Edward Honaker, a man convicted for rape on the basis of testimony
from the victim and her boyfriend. The prosecution never turned over an officer’s report that the
victim had not been “allowed to clearly see the [perpetrator] during the entire sequence of
events,” nor, more incredulously, did it reveal that the victim and her boyfriend were hypnotized
four months after the crime, at which time they first identified Honaker’s photo as that of the
rapist (Gould, 2007: 104). Instead, the prosecution’s witnesses were permitted to testify at trial,
identifying Honaker, without the defense being aware that there were good grounds to doubt any
identification. In cases like these, it is easy to see how the prosecution’s failure to disclose
material exculpatory evidence can lead to an erroneous conviction.
19
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

I.B.7 Inadequate Defense Representation
Even if prosecutors fail in their duties, we expect a suspect’s attorney to zealously
investigate and defend his case. As Bernhard (2001: 227-228) explains, “[i]t [is] the defense
counsel’s responsibility to protect [the innocent] from the mistakes of others: from witnesses’
misidentifications, police officers’ rush to judgment, and prosecution’s reluctance to reveal
potentially exculpatory material.” Yet, as a Columbia University study of capital appeals found,
ineffective defense lawyering was the biggest contributing factor to the erroneous conviction or
death sentence of criminal defendants in capital cases over a 23-year period (Liebman et al.,
2000). The central reason behind ineffective representation is inadequate funding, an absence of
quality control, and a lack of motivation (American Bar Association, 2006). The attorney may
be so rushed that he fails to communicate with his client or communicates “in a dismissive,
callous or hurried manner” (Berry, 2003: 490). He may make perfunctory attempts at discovery,
if any; engage in a narrow or shallow investigation; neglect to retain needed experts or test
physical evidence; fail to prepare for trial; or offer “weak trial advocacy and superficial or
tentative cross-examination” (Berry, 2003: 490). The result is a cascade of errors that dilutes or
even destroys the barrier provided by an effective advocate between an innocent defendant and
an erroneous conviction.
I.B.8. Interrelated Themes
Although the factors just discussed are those that appear most often in research on the
sources of wrongful conviction, three other issues merit mention for, if not definitive sources,
they serve as either background influences or interrelated factors. These include questions of
race, inadequate post-conviction remedies, and the role of the media. Any student of the
criminal justice system recognizes that there are serious race effects in the identification,
20
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

prosecution, and sentencing of criminal suspects. Racial and ethnic minorities are
disproportionately more likely than whites to be stopped and arrested by the police (Harris,
1999), and once convicted, they are also more likely to receive longer prison terms than do
whites (Mitchell, Haw, Pfeifer, & Meissner, 2005). They are also more likely to be subject to
some of the sources that lead to erroneous convictions (Martin, 2002). The clearest example is
mistaken eyewitness identification, in which the research indicates that errors are more likely
when the victim and perpetrator are of different races. In the cases studied, the most common
pattern of error is when a white victim is raped by an African-American or Hispanic man and
unintentionally identifies an innocent person as the perpetrator.
Another area of concern is jury decision-making; in a number of cases, all-white juries
have erroneously convicted African-American men based on questionable evidence and with
scant deliberation (Gould, 2007). To be sure, these problems are hardly limited to cases of
known exonerations; indeed, justice is threatened whenever a trier of fact allows racial
assumptions or prejudice to enter into his or her calculus. But when jurors (and judges) operate
on either known or even unconscious biases to convict the innocent, the legitimacy of the
criminal justice system is under even greater threat.
Once convicted, innocent defendants often find it extremely arduous to establish their
blamelessness. Legal doctrine makes such showings difficult to prove, for in throwing out a
conviction and, in some cases, ordering a new trial, the courts must be persuaded that no
reasonable juror (or judge in a bench trial) could have concluded that the defendant was guilty
(House v. Bell, 2006). The defendant’s task is even more onerous when states maintain
procedures, as Virginia did for decades, that a motion for new trial based on exculpating
evidence must be filed within 21 days of the order of conviction. As any criminal trial lawyer

21
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

knows, it is rare to the point of impossible for important, new evidence to come to light within
three weeks of sentencing.
Virginia has now replaced its twenty-one-day rule with a Writ of Actual Innocence
(Gould, 2007), a procedural outlet that other states are considering. But a promising law on the
books does not necessarily translate to actual exonerations if the courts that administer the law
are systematically skeptical of non-biological evidence. Indeed, it is hardly coincidental that the
vast majority of exonerations were achieved not because the courts stepped in and ordered a new
trial or habeas corpus relief, but because governors or other political leaders, including parole
boards, intervened. In some cases, they had the active support of prosecutors, who admirably
came forward to rectify what they believed had been a miscarriage of justice. But as Daniel
Medwed’s (2012) research also has shown, the institutional culture of some prosecutors’ offices
creates an environment in which “resistance to post-conviction innocence claims is an accepted
and pervasive cultural norm” that helps prosecutors avoid being seen as soft on crime (2004:
130). In such cases, an innocent but convicted defendant faces even greater obstacles in
rectifying the error done to him.
Finally, it is important to note the role of the media in both creating the conditions for
erroneous convictions and investigating doubtful cases post-conviction to help defendants prove
their innocence. One of the background conditions that raises the possibility of an erroneous
conviction is the heinousness of the underlying crime. Brutal rapes and murders, multiple
murders, and crimes against children particularly inflame the sensibilities of the public and
understandably lead to calls to catch and punish the criminal as quickly as possible. When these
crimes also generate press coverage—especially the sensational television coverage—authorities
are pressured to “do something” to apprehend a suspect. Under these circumstances, research

22
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

shows, police officers and prosecutors may feel rushed to complete their investigations and,
resultantly, may fall prey to tunnel vision that has them pursuing the wrong suspect (Findley &
Scott, 2006).
At the same time, the media, or more specifically, print reporters, have been instrumental
in establishing the innocence of some defendants who otherwise would have spent years in
prison, if not faced the prospect of execution. Perhaps the most famous of these reporters are
former journalists, now Northwestern University professors, David Protess and Robert Warden,
whose investigations with their students helped to uncover errors in several Illinois cases. They
were aided by Ken Armstrong, Steve Mills, and Maurice Possley, all writers for the Chicago
Tribune, whose “exposé” on erroneous capital convictions in Illinois was instrumental in
convincing then-Governor George Ryan to commute the sentences of Illinois’s death row
population and to issue a moratorium on further capital prosecutions until additional reforms
could be considered. Warden has written about this process and the power that investigative
journalism can have in raising awareness of wrongful convictions and building the constituency
for reform (Warden, 2003). As his Center on Wrongful Convictions (Leo & Gould, 2010: 858)
explains,
[i]t wasn’t that Americans didn’t care that innocent men and women were rotting in
prison or on death row, but rather that most people simply couldn’t accept the fact that
such miscarriages of justice could happen on a large scale. When the public and the
legal profession finally did come to recognize the alarming scope of the problem, it
turned out that there was a great deal of interest.

I.C. New Methods: Social Science Research on What Distinguishes Erroneous Convictions
from Other Cases

As useful as the research to date has been in uncovering and exploring potential sources
of erroneous convictions, its findings have largely been limited by the nature and scope of
23
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

inquiry. Based initially on single case studies, researchers were unable to compare the factors
that arose in a particular case of erroneous conviction with those that were found in other
exonerations. More significantly, with few exceptions, researchers have not collected data on
suitable control groups of cases. Even in those studies with control groups, the underlying
crimes at issue have generally been serious felonies, often capital matters. As a result, while the
field has been able to highlight particular sources of erroneous convictions, it has been unable to
say with certainty whether these factors apply to a multitude of erroneous conviction cases or
whether they are shared by other prosecutions that end in different results, including, for
example, accurate convictions or acquittals. Put another way, we are still left wondering whether
the sources identified to date are correlates or causes of erroneous convictions and if they are
contributing or exclusive sources (Leo, 2005; Gould, 2007).
This should not be surprising given that much of the research to date has been conducted
by more traditional legal scholars and journalists, who have tended to view erroneous
convictions through law’s more simplified model of cause and effect: an erroneous conviction
occurred, a cause is presumed, and the trigger is sought in order to prevent its harmful effects in
the future. Indeed, that is the very basis of both criminal and tort law, which hold that wrongs
have causes, that causes can be prevented, and that injuries from unacceptable causes warrant
recompense to the victim and punishment to the wrongdoer.
Only recently have researchers applied social science methods to study erroneous
convictions, a prospect that offers a better model to understand the sources of error. Social
science, of course, is primarily concerned with understanding the world as it is rather than as it
ought to be. The goal of traditional social science is generalizable knowledge. Empirical social
scientists draw on five primary methods of data gathering—experiments, field observation,

24
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

surveys, interviews, and analysis of documents—to produce valid and reliable knowledge about
social phenomena. Social scientists study cases and narratives as well, but the researchers are
more likely to describe these as sources of data rather than as a method. They are also more
likely to study narratives to discern generalizable patterns and explanations across cases (Leo &
Gould, 2009).
Empirical social science also seeks to identify more precisely the causal relationships
found in social phenomena. However, social scientists recognize that studying causation can be
elusive. Observed patterns or events may indicate that two variables are associated or co-vary
because they are correlated rather than because one causes the other. Strictly speaking, social
scientists can only infer causal relationships with certainty through experiments or randomized
control trials in which they can randomly assign subjects, introduce a stimulus on the
experimental group (and a placebo on the control group), and measure the independent effect of
the stimulus on the observed outcome. In the study of many social phenomena—including most
aspects of erroneous conviction—however, it is not possible to conduct meaningful experiments
for ethical and/or logistical reasons (e.g., researchers cannot expose research subjects to coercive
interrogations in order to induce false confessions). But, as we will discuss in greater depth
below, social scientists (and a few non-traditional legal scholars) have relied on other empirical
approaches to attempt to make more precise causal statements about patterns and risk factors in
the study of erroneous convictions.
I.C.1. Social Science Approaches
Considering that erroneous convictions are brought as actual cases, and recognizing that
the investigation and prosecution of these cases show divergent paths, a helpful approach for
studying them is controlled case comparisons. This method requires researchers to identify two
25
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

separate groups of cases that share independent variables (such as type of crime, prior felony
record, etc.) in order to, in effect, control for the potential explanatory effect of these influences
on different case outcomes. Done well, this method would allow scholars to more accurately
determine what factors are uniquely present in erroneous conviction cases, as well as to
statistically test hypotheses about what factors may be causally related to or predict erroneous
convictions. For example, researchers could compare a set of erroneous conviction cases against
a set of rightful acquittal cases or with a set of accurate conviction cases. The former would
explore the differences between cases where some innocent defendants are convicted and others
are acquitted; the latter would allow scholars to more systematically test the differences between
cases in which the innocent are convicted and those in which the guilty are convicted.
So far, there have been four studies of wrongful convictions using case comparisons, two
by criminologists (Harmon, 2001; Harmon & Lofquist, 2005) and, more recently, two by nontraditional criminal law scholars (Garrett, 2008; Gross & O’Brien, 2008). In the first study, Talia
Harmon assembled a data set of 76 cases from 1970 to 1998 in which death row prisoners were
exonerated and released. She also assembled a comparison, random data set of inmates
“convicted at trial and executed, from the same [jurisdictions and years] and in numbers
comparable to those of the inmates who were released from death row” (2001: 958). Harmon
used logistic regression equations to test which factors (i.e., independent variables) predicted
judicial exonerations in capital cases, finding that the discovery of new evidence, allegations of
perjury, and type of attorney were all statistically significant predictors of judicial exonerations
in capital cases. Harmon found that the amount of evidence introduced at trial was also a
statistically significant predictor of exonerations—namely that fewer types of evidence were
used in capital cases that eventually resulted in exonerations.

26
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

In a second study, Harmon and criminologist William Lofquist (2005) compared not the
innocent to the guilty in capital cases, but the innocent to the innocent—eighty-one judicial
exonerations of innocent death row prisoners to sixteen executions of death row prisoners whom
they believed to be innocent. Their goal was to identify factors that would statistically predict
case outcomes of death row prisoners with strong claims of factual innocence, e.g., why some
individuals wrongly convicted of capital crimes were exonerated while others were executed. As
in Harmon’s previous study, Harmon and Lofquist used a logistic regression model to test
several hypotheses. They found that allegations of perjury, multiple types of evidence, a prior
felony record, type of attorney at trial, and the race of the defendant were all significant
predictors of case outcomes. In short, defendants who had a private or resource center lawyer
representing them at trial (as opposed to a public defender) were significantly more likely to
have their capital conviction (correctly) overturned and be exonerated than be (erroneously)
executed. The same was true for convicted capital defendants whose prosecutors relied on fewer
forms of evidence at trial, who raised allegations of perjury on appeal, who did not have a prior
felony record, or whose case involved an African-American defendant and a white victim. Of
course, in Harmon and Lofquist’s study the assumptions of innocence were made by the
researchers, not by courts or other government officials.
Perhaps the most comprehensive study of wrongful convictions using a comparison
sample method is Brandon Garrett’s (2008) analysis of the first 200 innocent prisoners who were
released after post-conviction DNA testing exonerated them. Of these, Garrett selected the 121
non-capital cases that contained a written decision and assembled a comparison group of 121
non-capital cases that lacked DNA evidence showing innocence or guilt. These cases were
randomly selected using a Westlaw search to find all cases that had a published decision from the

27
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

same state and same year as the DNA exoneration cases and involved a conviction for the same
crime. Unlike the studies by Harmon and Lofquist, Garrett sought to understand how the
criminal justice system handled the cases of persons erroneously convicted but eventually
exonerated by post-conviction DNA testing. The comparison sample, thus, provided a non-DNA
control group of sorts, but, as Garrett points out, he did not know how many in the comparison
group were innocent. In addition, the comparison group contained less available information
about the evidence supporting the convictions because there were no news reports about these
cases, unlike in the DNA exonerations in which many were high profile.
Ironically, the real import of Garrett’s study for wrongful conviction scholars comes not
so much from the comparison sample, but from what his descriptive statistics of the first 200
DNA exonerations tell us: namely, that courts repeatedly misjudged the defendants’ innocence
and that lawyers, existing legal procedures, and actual innocence all failed to prevent these
individuals from being convicted. Garrett also describes why this occurred. As he notes (2008:
131), “[t]hese exonerees could not effectively litigate their factual innocence, likely due to a
combination of unfavorable legal standards, unreceptive courts, faulty criminal investigation by
law enforcement, inadequate representation at trial or afterwards, and a lack of resources for
factual investigation that might have uncovered miscarriages.”
Samuel Gross and Barbara O’Brien (2008) have also compared a sample of 105 cases of
capital defendants who were sentenced to death and exonerated between 1976 and 2003 with a
random sample of 137 executions carried out in the same period. Gross and O’Brien raise the
question: What is unique about capital cases that lead to exonerations versus those that lead to
execution? This is the same question Talia Harmon (2001) asked and analyzed seven years
earlier with a similar data set.

28
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Gross and O’Brien’s (2008) analysis implicitly assumes that the convicted capital
defendants who were executed are guilty and the ones who were exonerated are innocent,
although they are careful to qualify this point by attributing judgments about sufficiency of
evidence to the legal system. Unlike Harmon and Lofquist’s (2005) study (which compared the
innocent executed to the innocent exonerated), but like Harmon’s (2001) study, Gross and
O’Brien (2008) compare the innocent exonerated to the guilty executed. Using chi square tests
rather than regression models, Gross and O’Brien identify several statistically significant
differences between the capital convictions leading to exoneration and those leading to
execution. Defendants who were exonerated were significantly less likely to be reported as
mentally ill, more likely to have been tried for crimes that involve two or less victims, more
likely to have been tried for crimes that involve children as victims, less likely to have confessed,
more likely to have claimed innocence at trial, and more likely to have had an extensive criminal
record (especially violent felonies). In addition, in the capital cases leading to exonerations, the
time from crime to arrest was significantly much longer than in the cases leading to execution.
Gross and O’Brien’s analysis demonstrates that these differences are modest predictors of
exoneration in capital cases.
I.D. Investigating a New Question: How Erroneous Convictions Differ from Near Misses
As each of the four studies indicates, it is essential to create a comparison, or control,
group of cases when studying erroneous convictions. Otherwise, researchers risk labeling certain
sources as “causes” of erroneous convictions when the factors are merely correlates. The task,
again, is to understand how erroneous convictions differ from the other set of cases – which
sources are exclusive to erroneous convictions as against a different set of cases – and to develop
a better comprehension of how those sources interact with one another. In choosing a
29
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

comparison group, researchers typically assemble cases that differ in their exposure to a
particular condition. In medical research, for example, one group of patients may receive a new
drug while the control group takes a placebo. By contrast, experimental research in criminal
justice raises serious ethical and constitutional questions,1 so researchers often select a
comparison group that differs from the cases of interest on other bases, including outcome. In
Harmon and Lofquist’s (2005) study, for example, defendants in both sets of cases were
presumed to be innocent and differed only in the outcome of the matter; in one set of cases,
defendants were exonerated, and in the other they were executed.
The choice of a comparison group should speak to the underlying question at issue in the
research. In Harmon and Lofquist’s work, the researchers were interested in why the criminal
justice system exonerated some erroneously convicted capital defendants while executing others
who were also innocent. Gross and O’Brien (2008), by contrast—and Harmon herself (2001)—
sought to understand what distinguished the cases of innocent capital defendants who were
exonerated from those of guilty capital defendants who were executed. Garrett (2008), who does
not claim to have employed social science methods, set out to identify factors specific to the
innocent who were exonerated by DNA.2 At this point, social science research on erroneous
convictions is still too young to have evidenced a pattern of interest when employing a control
group, but the prior case studies of wrongful conviction may even ask a slightly different
question: in scouring the facts of individual cases of erroneous conviction, these studies seek to
understand which factors distinguish accurate from mistaken convictions.

                                                            
1

Neither the courts nor human subjects protection policies would permit a study, for example, in which one set of
subjects received the traditional Miranda warnings from the police while a second group was refused these
safeguards.
2
A difficulty in Garrett’s work is that his comparison group of cases may include guilty and innocent defendants.

30
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

I.E. How Does the Criminal Justice System Identify Innocence Before Conviction?
The foci of past studies are certainly valid, but missing so far in the literature is a study
that asks how the criminal justice system identifies innocent defendants in order to prevent
erroneous convictions. That is the subject of this study, which compares erroneous convictions
with “near misses.” In both sets of cases, an innocent defendant entered the criminal justice
system following indictment or information.3 In one set, the prosecution continued to an
erroneous conviction; in the other, the case against the defendant was dismissed, or the defendant
was acquitted at trial, on the basis of factual innocence.
This is an essential distinction and one that so far has lacked scholarly attention. Cases of
erroneous conviction, of course, are the most dramatic examples of how the system got the most
important thing—the guilt or innocence of the defendant—wrong. They illustrate a breakdown
in the accuracy of human judgment at multiple levels: police investigation, prosecution, pre-trial
motions, judicial rulings, and ultimately jury verdicts. Near misses, by contrast, illustrate how
the criminal justice process got it right (or at least how the court system got it right) in acquitting
or dismissing charges against a factually innocent person.4 What we want to know—and thus
what dictated our research strategy—is what factors are uniquely present in the cases that lead
the system to rightfully acquit or dismiss charges against the innocent that are not present in
cases that lead the system to erroneously convict the innocent. If we understand this, then it may
                                                            
3

Throughout this report we simplify the discussion to say by indictment. This expression should be read to include
those felonies initiated by information.
4
We choose to focus on post-indictment cases in this study for both theoretical and practical reasons. Theoretically,
it makes sense to compare indictments and convictions, because presumably both are an official recognition of the
level of evidence indicating the defendant’s guilt; this is in contrast to earlier stages in the process, such as arrest or
interrogation, that are more informal and often about public safety concerns or simple fact-gathering. Practically, we
do not have access to sufficient information about pre-indictment cases to use these cases in a thorough comparison,
as we conducted here. At the same time, we acknowledge that there are many cases of innocent defendants who are
correctly “weeded out” of the system prior to indictment, and that in this study we are unable to detect the variance
between cases that are discarded before indictment and those that are indicted (for example, if police are more likely
to discard investigations against white suspects than black suspects).

31
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

be but a short step to comprehending what policy interventions can influence the justice system
to get it right and acquit the innocent, thereby preventing future erroneous convictions.
This project differs from past research in four other respects. First, unlike the death
penalty studies (Harmon/Lofquist/Gross) that matched alleged exonerations in capital cases to
non-exonerations in death penalty cases, we relied on a stricter definition of factual innocence.
As we note above, one of the problems with the Harmon/Lofquist/Gross line of research is that
they do not know whether the “exoneration” cases are truly innocent or whether the “nonexoneration” cases in their comparison sample are truly guilty. For that matter, even in the
Garrett (2008) study, which is an improvement over prior work, the author could not say what
percentage of the control group was truly innocent.
Second, this project encompasses a wider range of violent felonies and is not limited
simply to capital cases. Third, our study had a greater sample size than the earlier
Harmon/Lofquist/Garrett/Gross and O’Brien line of studies: as will be discussed in more detail
later in this report, we analyzed 460 cases total (260 erroneous convictions and 200 near misses).
Finally, we were able to employ advanced statistical tests that allow a more detailed analysis of
the cases than descriptive (Garrett) or chi-square (Gross and O’Brien) statistics.
I.E.1. Hypotheses: Comparing Erroneous Convictions and Near Misses
(i) Introduction
Comparing erroneous convictions to near misses not only will help to explain how and
why innocent persons are convicted but also should aid in creating predictors for how the
criminal justice system identifies innocent defendants, and thus, prevents erroneous convictions.
Although there have been no prior descriptive or comparative studies of near misses, the research
literature on erroneous conviction allows us to posit and test a number of hypotheses about what
32
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

our study comparing erroneous convictions with near misses may show. The core issue, of
course, is why some innocents were erroneously convicted whereas others had their cases
dismissed or were acquitted at trial. Four sets of hypotheses emerge: one having to do with the
traditional sources of error that are often treated as “causes” of erroneous conviction; one set of
hypotheses having to do with sociological factors that are believed to influence erroneous
convictions; one set of hypotheses having to do with the structure of the investigative and
adjudicative phases of the adversary criminal justice system; and one set of hypotheses having to
do with the different actors, and actions they take, within the criminal justice system.
(ii) Hypotheses Concerning the Traditional Legal Sources of Error
The first set of hypotheses we posit involves the traditional legal sources of error that we
have reviewed above. Researchers have routinely treated these types of errors as “causes” of
erroneous conviction, and sometimes, implicitly or explicitly, as seemingly the most salient or
impactful cause of erroneous conviction. For example, some researchers have stated that
eyewitness identification testimony is the strongest evidence that the government can bring
against an accused (Loftus & Ketcham, 1991). Similar claims have been made about confession
evidence, which has been described as “uniquely potent,” “highly prejudicial,” and as often
“trump[ing] innocence” (Kassin, 2012: 2). Accordingly, our first set of hypotheses would be that
the seven traditional legal sources of error described above are more likely to be present in the
erroneous conviction exonerations than in the near misses:


Eyewitness error will be significantly more likely in the erroneous convictions than in the
near misses;



False confessions will be significantly more likely in erroneous convictions than in the
near misses;
33
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.



Knowingly false informant testimony or evidence will be significantly more likely in
erroneous convictions than in near misses;



Forensic error will be significantly more likely in erroneous convictions than in near
misses;



Tunnel vision will be significantly more likely in erroneous convictions than in near
misses;



Prosecutorial error will be significantly more likely in erroneous convictions than in near
misses; and



Defendants in erroneous convictions will be significantly more likely to have received a
weaker defense than those in near misses.

(iii) Hypotheses Concerning Sociological Factors Believed to Influence Erroneous Conviction
These seven “traditional” sources of error are sometimes juxtaposed against more
sociological sources of error, such as race, class, or demographic effects (Bedau & Radelet,
1987; Free & Ruesink, 2012). The idea here is that minorities and people from lower socioeconomic status or class background are at greater risk of poor treatment in the criminal justice
system, which may increase the chance of an erroneous conviction. The heightened risk may
operate directly—where, for example, racial minorities are arrested at a higher rate than whites in
America (Tonry, 1995)—or the danger may be indirect, such as prior findings that African
Americans who murder white victims are disproportionately sentenced to death (Baldus,
Woodworth, & Pulaski, 1990). In addition, given that criminal justice practices operate within a

34
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

larger political system and culture, we anticipate regional differences in the prevalence and
distribution of erroneous convictions.5 Accordingly, we hypothesize that:


Racial minorities are significantly more likely to be present in the erroneous convictions
than in the near misses;



African Americans charged with crimes against white victims are significantly more
likely to be present in the erroneous convictions than in the near misses;



Cross-racial misidentification is significantly more likely to be present in erroneous
convictions than in near misses;



Class effects are significantly more likely to be present in the erroneous convictions than
in the near misses (i.e., individuals from low socio-economic status are significantly
more likely to be present in the erroneous convictions than in the near misses);



Individuals with prior criminal records, especially prior felony records and/or numerous
prior convictions, are significantly more likely to be present in the erroneous convictions
than in the near misses;



Erroneous convictions are significantly more likely to occur in former confederate states
than in the northern, western and mid-western states;



Erroneous convictions are significantly more likely to occur in states with a strong
attachment to the death penalty or more punitive culture than near misses;6

                                                            
5

In a study of federal capital defense, researchers identified substantial regional differences in the legal and expert
resources provided to defendants (Gould & Greenman, 2010).
6
Many of the states with the highest number of exonerations are also states with large death rows (Garrett, 2008: 67;
Gross et al., 2005: 541). In addition, it appears that exoneration rates for death sentences are much higher than for
other murder convictions and for criminal convictions generally. The connection between the use of the death
penalty and wrongful convictions may be the result of the fact that capital cases are sometimes afforded greater legal
protections (such as increased opportunities to appeal or additional counsel) and are often highly scrutinized by a
strong local activist community. Id. The greater scrutiny and incentives may result in more erroneous convictions
being discovered. If this is the case, then the death penalty is not a source or predictor of the occurrence of erroneous
convictions, but rather simply related to how these cases are uncovered. Id. However, an alternative explanation for
the relationship between the death penalty and higher rates of erroneous convictions is that the death penalty is

35
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.



Erroneous convictions are significantly more likely than near misses to occur in states
that are politically conservative;



Erroneous convictions are significantly more likely than near misses to occur in states in
which there are high or rising crime rates;



Erroneous convictions are significantly more likely to be “high profile” cases than near
misses, not only representing more serious crimes but also generating greater media
attention;

(iv) Hypotheses Concerning Case Characteristics and the Structure of the Investigative and
Adjudicative Process in the Adversary System
Criminal cases in the American adversary system move from the investigative phase to
the adjudicative phase and then to the sentencing and post-conviction phases. Some wrongful
conviction researchers have argued that errors are more likely to occur in the investigative phase
(Simon, 2012) and that once these errors occur they are likely to snowball, making it more
difficult to reverse the errors at each subsequent step or stage of the criminal process (Leo,
2008). Researchers have also argued that erroneous convictions tend to result from multiple, not
singular, errors. These observations suggest that it is easier to reverse or prevent an erroneous
conviction the earlier one intervenes in the process, and that the fewer the errors present,
especially cumulative errors, the less likely a person is to be erroneously convicted. We can
posit several hypotheses based on case characteristics and the structure of the American
adversary system of criminal justice:


Police errors are significantly more likely in erroneous convictions than in near misses;

                                                                                                                                                                                                
indicative of a local legal culture that is violent and punitive and often historically racist (see discussions in
Kutateladze (2009) and Cochran et al. (2006); see also Clarke (1998) and Holmes (1992) on the legacy of lynching).
These traits may make police, prosecutors, and the community in such jurisdiction more likely to seek conviction
despite evidence of innocence.

36
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.



Erroneous convictions are significantly more likely to have multiple errors than near
misses;



The time between crime and arrest will be longer in erroneous convictions than near
misses; and



The evidence against defendants will be stronger in erroneous convictions than near
misses.

(v) Hypotheses Concerning Actors and Actions within the Criminal Justice System
It is a truism of sociology, and social science more generally, that all behavior can be
explained as a result of the interaction between structure and agency. In the context of erroneous
convictions, the actions of criminal justice officials, particularly police, prosecutors and defense
attorney, can have a huge impact on whether or not a particular case leads to an erroneous
conviction. Many hypotheses involving these effects are named above as well. We posit the
following additional hypotheses with respect to the behavior of criminal justice officials:


Brady violations are significantly more likely in erroneous convictions than in near
misses;



Intentional error is significantly more likely in erroneous convictions than in near misses;
by contrast, unintentional error is significantly more likely in near misses than in
erroneous convictions.

37
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

II. METHODS
To address these hypotheses and investigate the overarching questions that informed
them, this study used a mixed methods approach that involved both quantitative and qualitative
analysis. We began by identifying a combined set of erroneous conviction and near miss cases
that met the project’s criteria. After researching and coding the cases along a number of
variables, we analyzed the cases using bivariate and logistic regression techniques. With the
assistance of an expert panel, we also explored the cases from a qualitative perspective and
examined the statistical results in light of this exploration. Below, we explain in more detail how
we identified cases and coded case facts. Then, in the following two sections, we discuss the
results of the quantitative and qualitative analyses.
II.A. Case Criteria
Each case in our study involved a factually innocent defendant who was indicted (or,
where applicable, charged by information) by a state for a violent felony against a person and
was subsequently relieved of all legal responsibility for the crime. Two categories were created:
“erroneous convictions” for those who were exonerated after conviction and “near misses” for
those who were acquitted or had charges dismissed before conviction on the basis of factual
innocence.
The project employed a conservative definition of factual innocence that clearly
distinguishes factual innocence from innocence based on procedural error or other purely legal
criteria (so-called “legal innocence”). The former means that someone else committed the crime.
The latter refers to cases in which the defendant's indictment or conviction was overturned
because the state violated his fundamental rights or otherwise made a legal error (Gould & Leo

38
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

2010). While a legally innocent defendant may also be factually innocent of the crime, this is
not always true. In each case analyzed in this study, we relied on factual innocence.
To establish factual innocence, each case had to possess two separate components:
(1)

a judicial, executive or legislative acknowledgement that the individual did not
commit the crime for which he was erroneously indicted (including a statement of
innocence by a prosecutor, governor, judge, state compensation board, or a juror
after an acquittal), and

(2)

evidence that would convince a reasonable person that the individual did not
commit the crime (such as post-conviction DNA testing, another individual was
prosecuted and convicted of the crime, or a new diagnosis of the victim’s
condition).

In general, a prosecutor’s decision not to retry a defendant after a judge overturned the
defendant’s conviction was not, by itself, considered a sufficient statement of innocence to
include the case in our study. However, in a few rare situations where the evidence was
uncontroverted and strong enough to prove beyond any doubt that the defendant was factually
innocent, we did not require the case to have an official statement of innocence. These were
exclusively single-defendant rape cases in which DNA tests on the semen excluded the
defendant as the contributor.
We considered it important to employ such a conservative definition of factual innocence
for several reasons. First, other research studies on erroneous convictions have suffered from
lengthy debates about the actual innocence of some of the defendants included in their study.
This, in turn, detracts from a meaningful discussion about the majority of cases over which there
is no debate. In addition, one of the goals of the current research was to involve criminal justice

39
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

practitioners from traditionally opposing sides that have not previously participated in a common
project. To ensure the cooperation of these opposing sides, we needed to employ a definition
that was acceptable to the majority of experts and practitioners.
In keeping with our definition that distinguishes factual from legal innocence, we did not
include cases in which the defendant actually committed the underlying action of the alleged
offense (the actus reus) but lacked the necessary mental state (the mens rea). These included
cases with a successful plea of self-defense or insanity. Such cases rely on legal reasoning and
standards of culpability rather than on the factual question of whether the defendant committed
the criminal act, and the types of errors involved are also likely distinct. We did, however,
include cases of alleged rape where it was later determined that the parties had consensual sex.
In these cases the defendant was factually innocent because part of the offense—the lack of
consent or force—did not occur.
Cases had to involve a violent felony against a person. We considered this to be the most
appropriate type of case for our study because it balanced competing interests. On the one hand,
we were sensitive to a bias that has been noted in recent erroneous conviction literature—that is,
many studies focus on identifying and studying primarily rapes and murder, which are likely
unrepresentative of the majority of erroneous convictions (Garrett, 2008; Gross, 2005). On the
other hand, in order to succeed our research required cases that either had official documents on
file or would be memorable to those who participated in the case. Less serious felonies and
misdemeanors unfortunately do not meet these criteria. Thus, by focusing on violent felonies
against a person, we strove to expand our sample to include crimes beyond murder and rape
without losing the ability to conduct thorough research.

40
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Qualifying offenses included murder in any degree, voluntary manslaughter, attempted
murder, aggravated assault, rape or other sexual assaults involving penetration, attempted rape,
and robbery. Aggravation included kidnapping and the use of a weapon. Other felonies, such as
kidnapping or arson, were included if the underlying situations included the use of a weapon or
resulted in significant bodily harm to an individual. Aiding and abetting either murder or rape
was also included if the defendant was alleged to have been at the scene of the crime.
The defendant must have been convicted or indicted no earlier than January 1, 1980 and
exonerated, acquitted, or dismissed no later than January 1, 2012. We chose these dates to be
inclusive as possible while still ensuring that the law enforcement techniques at issue in the cases
were relevant to today and that case files or information were still accessible. The project
considered cases from all 50 state jurisdictions but excluded federal and military tribunal cases.
Not only are the vast majority of criminal prosecutions in the United States conducted within the
states, but the resources available to prosecutors and defendants in federal cases are unlike those
in state cases (Cole & Smith, 2010).
A few defendants were included more than once in the database because they had
indictments in two or more jurisdictions for separate crimes or had more than one independent
case in a single jurisdiction.
II.B. Identifying Qualifying Cases
Researchers systematically identified potential cases, under a common method, using
multiple approaches. These included: examining prior publications in the field; searching
internet websites and blogs; investigating media coverage of these incidents using online
newspaper databases, such as Google Archives; and soliciting potential cases through national
outreach to lawyers, criminal justice officials, local innocence projects, and scholars. Using
41
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

these methods, researchers identified a total of 628 potential erroneous convictions and over 400
potential near misses.
While the same methods were used to identify both sets of cases, the acquittals and
dismissals proved more difficult to identify. In contrast to the often well-documented and
consolidated information available for many erroneous convictions, there are no databases,
articles, or books written specifically about acquittals and dismissals based on innocence, and no
innocence projects take these cases. Therefore, the project relied more heavily on case
solicitation from legal practitioners and scholars, media research, and scholarly articles written
on specific sources of error (such as false confessions or junk science). To that end, researchers
solicited potential near misses through mass emails to members of the National District
Attorney’s Association, the Association of Prosecuting Attorneys, the National Association of
Criminal Defense Lawyers, and state-wide defense associations. Researchers also worked with
members of the National Institute of Justice to solicit cases at the agency’s annual conference
and through word of mouth. To facilitate responses from the case solicitations, the project
created a confidential online case submission form that could be accessed through the project’s
website.
Once the potential erroneous convictions and near misses were identified, researchers
conducted a thorough assessment of the cases to determine if they met the project criteria, most
importantly that of factual innocence. Rather than relying on the assertions of others that a case
constituted an erroneous conviction or near miss, the project conducted extensive research into
case facts as well as contacted individuals who could confirm a statement of innocence.
Throughout this process, researchers eliminated more than half the cases initially identified
because they did not match the study’s selective criteria. The initial decision to include cases

42
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

was made by individual researchers. When uncertainty arose, the case was discussed among two
or more researchers, including the coding supervisor or the principal investigator. Finally, each
included case was then reviewed by the coding supervisor to ensure that it met the project’s
definition.
Both forms of near misses proved particularly challenging. Because they did not go to
trial, some dismissals, although they had a statement of innocence, lacked sufficient facts by
which researchers could independently verify that innocence. Even more challenging was
obtaining an official declaration of innocence in acquittal cases, since there is little reason for a
public official or juror to give such a statement after a defendant succeeds in court. For our
purposes, a single juror’s assertion of the defendant’s factual innocence, whether through a direct
interview with a project researcher or gathered through the media, was a sufficient statement of
innocence, and even this was extremely difficult to acquire. However, even if such a statement
of innocence could be obtained, we still needed case facts that would convince us—not just the
juror—that the defendant was innocent.
Because of the difficulty and time-intensive research required to identify qualifying
dismissals and acquittals, the project received approval from the National Institute of Justice to
reduce the target number of near misses from 250 to 200. In addition, dismissals were more
likely than acquittals to meet the study’s requirement of a statement of innocence, so the final set
of near misses was predominately dismissals.
In total, the project identified 260 erroneous convictions and 200 near misses that met the
project’s definition and were included in the study. Among the erroneous convictions, 87
percent (n=226) were jury trials, five percent (n=12) were bench trials, and seven percent (n=18)

43
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

were pleas.7 Among the near misses, 91 percent (n=182) were dismissals and the remaining nine
percent (n=18) were acquittals at a jury trial.
The geographical distribution of the cases is shown in Figure 1 and Tables 1-2. Both
types of cases are located throughout the United States and are relatively more frequent in
heavily populated urban areas. In particular, near misses are more common in states and
counties with large populations and/or high crime rates. This is what we would expect from an
unbiased collection of cases and suggests that our data are broadly representative of near misses.
By contrast, the distribution of erroneous convictions is only partially explainable by
population and crime rate. We found, as have other researchers, that there are unusually high
concentrations of these cases in areas such as Cook County, IL and Dallas County, TX. Scholars
have offered various explanations for this distribution (Zalman, 2012). Some of the
explanations, such as systemic police and prosecutorial misconduct, suggest that erroneous
convictions may actually occur at a higher rate in these jurisdictions. Other possible reasons,
such as the presence of well-established innocence projects or prosecutorial conviction integrity
units, and high media coverage in these jurisdictions, have more to do with the rate that cases are
uncovered and reported (Gross, 2005). Although our research does not directly speak to what
causes the geographical distributions of erroneous convictions and near misses, we can note that
these distributions do vary to some extent between the two types of cases. In addition, as
discussed later, we were able to employ statistical controls to account for any geographic
concentrations among the cases.

                                                            
7

To the best of our knowledge, none were Alford pleas. However, even if they were, we would still consider them
erroneous convictions. Alford pleas meet our definition in so far as they involve the conviction of a factually
innocent defendant for a crime he did not commit. Of course, reasonable minds differ as to whether this type of plea
should be allowed or encouraged in the criminal justice system (and therefore whether the conviction is “erroneous”
or “wrongful”).

44
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Figure 1 about here]

With 460 cases of erroneous convictions and near misses from across the United States,
our data set is unique within the literature on erroneous convictions for several reasons. First, it
is one of only a handful of projects that involves a control group (see Section I.C.1); our
control—the near misses—enables us to move beyond a description of erroneous convictions and
analyze what separates these cases from other cases of innocence. Second, the size of our data
set, as compared to previous studies with only 20 or 50 cases, allows us to run more robust
statistical analyses; in addition, because they involve non-capital crimes, our cases are also more
diverse than those in many previous studies. Together, this increases our confidence in the
representativeness of the results. Finally, the project’s conservative definition of innocence
ensures that the increase in number of cases does not come at the expense of an extremely solid
factual basis for categorizing the cases as we did and helps build broad engagement among
scholars and criminal justice practitioners in the conclusions.
II.C. Data Collection and Case Coding
II.C.1. The Narrative Coding Document
For each qualifying case, researchers initially collected data to answer 61 umbrella
questions on the project’s narrative coding document (see Appendix VIII.A). The questions
were designed to reflect possible sources of erroneous convictions as suggested by prior
research, as well as those factors that might distinguish erroneous convictions from dismissals
and acquittals based on innocence (see hypotheses in Section I.E.1). The questions involved
three separate but non-exclusive categories of case facts: demographics, procedural information,
and evidential information. The questions were largely open-ended and designed to elicit a
45
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

narrative response. For instance, under demographics the researcher was asked to describe any
previous arrests or convictions that the defendant had. Under procedural information, a question
asked whether the case had been appealed, and if so, what was the court’s decision and rationale.
In the evidential information section, the researcher was asked to list all eyewitnesses and the
relationship of each witness to the defendant. A few questions, such as race of the defendant,
had pre-designated choices for the researcher to answer.
To ensure inter-coder reliability among the researchers, the project created a coding book
that contained rules for answering each question. For instance, for the inquiry, “What was the
result of the first court proceeding in the case?” the coding rules gave the following explanation:
Choose from “guilty,” “acquittal,” “dismissal,” and “other.” “Other” includes hung juries
and mistrials. If there is more than one disposition, e.g., the defendant was acquitted of
one charge and found guilty of another, please indicate this by circling all that apply and
writing in an explanation. If the defendant is found guilty of a lesser included offense but
not the primary offense, indicate this as “guilty.” (Appendix VIII.B)

Throughout the process of coding, the book was updated regularly to address issues and
ambiguities as they arose.
As a further step to ensure inter-coder reliability, the project supervisor reviewed each
narrative coding sheet as it was completed by a researcher. If the supervisor determined that one
or more researchers were inconsistent in the way they answered a question on the sheet, she
brought this to the attention of all the researchers and, together, the project staff developed a
coding rule to address the problem.
II.C.2. Sources of Case Facts
The project used multiple sources to investigate cases and collect relevant case facts,
including case files, transcripts, other court records, scholarly articles and books, news reports,
and interviews with individuals familiar with the facts and context of the cases. In this work,
46
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

project staff was greatly aided by the Innocence Project, which made its archives available for
the research, and the Association of Prosecuting Attorneys, which helped to introduce
researchers to current and former prosecutors. Court or other governmental records were the
preferred sources for research, which were obtained either through the state institution itself
(such as searchable online court records, transcripts, and gubernatorial archives), or through the
Innocence Project’s database. The Innocence Project’s database included trial transcripts and
court proceedings for a subset of their cases. Researchers also relied upon LexisNexis Academia
to obtain any appellate court opinions (whether the criminal case itself or a subsequent civil suit)
relevant to the cases.
The researchers supplemented written documents with interviews of sources
knowledgeable about the cases. This method was especially crucial for the near misses, which
had far sparser case files than the erroneous convictions, but it also proved immensely helpful in
providing context and additional insight into documentary evidence uncovered through other
means. Interviewees included defense attorneys, prosecutors, journalists, police officers, former
judges, jurors, and others involved in the cases. Consistent with prior research conducted by the
principal investigator, researchers did not contact victims (Gould, 2007). The names and contact
information of possible interviewees for each case were identified through official documents or
secondary sources.8 Researchers distributed a template email and letter to potential interviewees
explaining the project, and informed consent was obtained before interviews began (see
Appendix VIII.D).
Interviews were designed to elicit specific facts about a case and to give subjects an
opportunity to speak more expansively about why they thought the relevant indictment had
                                                            
8

Interviews were usually conducted after the completion of some documentary research so that the researcher could
ask pertinent questions and so that the interviewee’s information could be verified through an outside source.

47
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

occurred and why the case had concluded as it did. Researchers were careful to distinguish the
reporting of facts and opinions; the former were used for case coding, whereas the latter helped
to provide context for the case outcome. The exception was when a prosecutor or judge related
that s/he believed the defendant had been factually innocent. In these cases, we used the
declaration to satisfy one prong of our definition of factual innocence. However, a defense
attorney’s opinion was not considered a statement of innocence.
Although official documents and interviews were prioritized over other methods of data
collection, some case facts were only obtainable through media and news sources. Therefore,
researchers conducted thorough searches through Google and Google Archives. Google
Archives was particularly useful in identifying articles from local newspapers as well as
supplementary material in newspapers (such as letters to the editor and photographs) that were
often not available from other sources. Finally, where available, researchers consulted additional
sources including academic articles, popular books and documentary films.
(i) Conflicting Versions of Facts and Missing Data
While case coding was generally straightforward and objective, occasionally researchers
were faced with discrepancies or missing data. In some instances, facts varied between the
sources, and researchers needed to determine which version of the facts should be used in the
coding process. Thus, an informal hierarchy of written sources was created to establish
uniformity between researchers. Official documents were considered the most accurate written
resource for case information, followed by academic articles and books, and finally newspapers
and Internet sources. For example, if a defendant appeared Hispanic and was referred to as such
in the media, but he was listed as Caucasian in the Department of Corrections records, the
researcher coded his race as Caucasian.

48
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

A more difficult situation arose when an interviewee contradicted a written source. In
this instance, the researcher was required to evaluate the trustworthiness and memory of the
interviewee. If the interviewee was deemed highly reliable, information gained via the interview
was given great weight, overriding differing opinions in all but court documents and scholarly
articles. On the other hand, if the interviewee was not deemed particularly reliable, information
taken from the interview would be treated as akin to a newspaper article. Again, an example
illustrates the point. In one case, several newspapers, while recognizing the defendant’s
innocence, described him as a gang member and part of an organized crime ring. However,
when researchers spoke with a local government official who was close to the case, the official
insisted this was not true. He claimed that the story about an organized gang was simply hype
surrounding a relatively high profile case. Because of the official’s close proximity to the case
facts and his obvious reason to answer otherwise (the defendant’s allegiance to a gang or
organized crime ring would have made the government’s erroneous indictment more
understandable), we deemed his opinion to be trustworthy. Thus, we did not code the defendant
as a gang member. If a researcher ultimately could not resolve a discrepancy or disagreement of
facts, she noted it in the narrative coding document and the answer was not used in statistical
analysis.
When a fact was not available using the sources listed above, researchers recorded it as
“unknown” on the narrative coding document and left it blank during statistical coding. The
hardest facts to obtain were certain types of demographic information, such as the number of
prior convictions or whether the defendant was a high school graduate. For a few variables, we
made limited assumptions when data were missing. Specifically, if no sources made note of the
defendant’s gang affiliation or suggested a situation in which membership would be plausible, it

49
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

was presumed that the defendant did not have an affiliation with any gang. Additionally, if our
research failed to uncover any evidence of a cognitive impairment or knowledge of another
language, we presumed the defendant did not have cognitive impairment or was reasonably
fluent in English, respectively.
II.C.3. Coding Case Facts into SPSS
Once researchers completed the coding document for each case, the cases were entered
into SPSS. Over 600 statistical variables were created to capture the 61 umbrella questions in
the narrative coding document. For example, the narrative question “Was there forensic
evidence in the case?” was reformulated into seven separate SPSS variables: (i) forensic
evidence, (ii) fingerprint/impression evidence, (iii) hair/fiber evidence, (iv) ballistics evidence,
(v) serology evidence, (vi) DNA evidence, and (vii) bitemark evidence. For every case, the
researcher had to code one of three possible answers for each of these variables: not presented
(=99), presented without errors (=0), or presented with errors (=1).
To facilitate the translation between the narrative coding document and SPSS and to
ensure inter-coder reliability, the project created a separate statistical coding book that defined
each of the SPSS variables (Appendix VIII.E). Prior to entering any cases into SPSS, the
researchers underwent thorough training on the use of the statistical program and the coding
book. Several test cases were also coded and discussed to ensure consistency. The project
supervisor and at least one additional researcher independently coded the cases into SPSS; the
coders then met weekly to compare how they coded each case. If there were a discrepancy
between the coders on any variable, it was resolved by referring back to the narrative coding
sheet (in the case of an entry error) or the coding book (in the case of a disagreement about how
to code). Because the process of inter-coder checking took over six months, the coders also
50
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

conducted random cross-checking on previously checked cases to ensure that coding was
consistent over time.
II.D. Rating the Strength of Cases
II.D.1. Introduction to the Police Foundation Rating Scale
The project applied to both sets of cases an instrument from the Police Foundation that
examines the strength of evidence available to police and prosecutors. The purpose was to allow
us to distinguish between “easy” cases, in which few people would have considered a defendant
to be guilty, and “harder” cases, where the facts of a case might have convinced many reasonable
people to believe the defendant was guilty even though he was innocent. Indeed, it is one thing
for an erroneous conviction to occur when the facts seem overwhelming – as, for example, when
an eyewitness is certain of her identification and the forensic evidence is consistent with guilt; it
may be quite another for those cases in which the identification procedure is dubious, the
forensic evidence fails to match, and the defendant has an alibi.
The project chose the Police Foundation’s scale as the most appropriate tool for this
research because, as the Foundation notes, it is the first evidentiary strength rating scale for
which “content-oriented validity evidence” has been generated.9 The scale was originally
                                                            
9

The Police Foundation scale improves upon previous research. One of the first measures of case strength was
developed by Bronx District Attorney Mario Merola (1982). Merola's system evaluated cases on four different
aspects: 1) the nature of the crime charged; 2) the gravity of the particular offense; 3) the propensity of the defendant
to commit crimes of violence (as indicated by the defendant's background and criminal record); and 4) the strength
of the case (Merola, 1982). Merola's system was largely subjective and little effort was made to validate the scale.
Later, Behrman and Davey (2001) examined archived eyewitness identifications and categorized the cases as having
no extrinsic evidence, or extrinsic evidence that was either minimally probative or substantially probative. Like
Merola, the scale lacked any content-oriented validation; in addition, because the scale was essentially dichotomous
if the case had extrinsic evidence, the quality of cases condensed into the minimally or substantially probative
categories varied drastically. As a result, the scale lacked precision and utility. The Police Foundation scale
improves upon the Merola (1982) and Behrman and Davey (2001) scales by generating exemplars that serve as
objective anchors, engaging experts in a process of content-oriented validity, and expanding the scale to include five
categories of evidential strength. The result is a more nuanced, objective, and applicable tool.

51
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

designed by the Foundation as a method of getting at “ground truth” (e.g., is the suspect really
guilty?) when assessing the effect of presentation format on the rate of false or correct
eyewitness identifications. The scale contains six categories of information or evidence; each
category in turn contains exemplars that have been given a rating on a 5-point scale (5 being
strong evidence, 1 being weak evidence). These exemplars serve as “anchors” for the
investigator or researcher when assessing how the investigator’s own case facts should be rated.
To create these exemplars and generate “content-oriented validity evidence,” the Police
Foundation surveyed a dozen subject matter experts to determine the strength of particular
examples of evidence. These original experts included four judges, three prosecutors, two
defense attorneys, four police investigators (two of whom were also research scientists), and one
criminal justice researcher/lawyer. If an example received a relatively consistent score by each
expert, it was assigned the average score and included as an exemplar in the scale. By contrast,
particular examples of evidence that received widely differing scores were not used in the scale.
As part of the multi-stage development process, the Police Foundation subsequently
refined the scale by having over five dozen other experts and practitioners rate the exemplars, as
well as through a pilot project at a police agency. The Foundation is currently in the process of
using the rating scale in research at several police agencies across the country and continues to
develop further evidence of content-oriented validity.
II.D.2. Applying the Police Foundation Rating Scale to Project Cases
(i) Modifications to the Scale
To increase inter-coder reliability and address the project’s unique research questions, we
made some minor changes in the scale before applying it to our cases. In consultation with the
Police Foundation, we simplified the tool by collapsing the 5-point scale into a 3-point scale.
52
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Exemplars that were originally given a score between 1 and 2.5 were given a new score of 1
(“weak evidence”), exemplars that were originally scored between 2.5 and 4.5 became a 2
(“probative evidence”), and exemplars in the 4.5 to 5 range were given a score of 3 (“highly
probative evidence”).
The project also created several new exemplars. In the category of physical evidence,
this was necessary because the original exemplars had been developed quite recently and did not
include common types of evidence frequently found in older cases (such as serology and hair
morphology). The project’s cases date back to 1980 and therefore some guidelines were
necessary for rating these “older” types of forensic evidence. The research team created and
rated the new exemplars with input from the Police Foundation. We also added exemplars to the
category of suspect statements. The original exemplars did not address many of the possible
factors that might correlate with a false confession, such as a mental disorder or “dream
confession.” The same process for creating new exemplars, as outlined above, was used in this
instance. In addition, we consulted with an expert on false confessions.
The last modification to the original Police Foundation scale was to separate the evidence
presented by the defense and prosecution for each case. Because the Police Foundation designed
the scale to be used only on information contained within a police file, its scale does not make
any distinction between evidence available to the state and that presented by the defense. The
project, on the other hand, had information from many different sources, often including defense
attorneys. In addition, one of the hypotheses of the research was that quality of defense would
affect case outcome; therefore, we considered it vital to create a new, seventh category that
evaluated the strength of the defense case. Researchers created exemplars of possible defense
evidence, modeled on those in the original scale, and gave the exemplars a rating. Following

53
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

consultation with the Police Foundation, the rated exemplars were then included in a new scale
for defense activity.
(ii) Training and Method of Rating
The Police Foundation trained four researchers (including the PI and project supervisor)
in the use of its rating tool. Training included two half-day sessions at the Police Foundation
with the head of research at the Foundation and an assistant. The Police Foundation explained
the creation and purpose of the scale, provided the research team with copies of the scale and
supporting documents, and answered questions from the researchers about implementing the
scale. After each training session, the researchers were in contact with the Police Foundation
with follow-up questions as the team began to implement the scale.
After discussions with the Police Foundation, we decided that initially each case should
be rated independently by three researchers (including the coding supervisor), without
consultation with the other researchers. Then, the researchers would compare their ratings.
Every rating had to be agreed upon by two out of the three researchers before it could be
recorded as final. In most cases, when the researchers compared their individual ratings, at least
two of the researchers rated the case identically, and this rating was recorded as final.
Occasionally, when the researchers did not agree, they consulted the PI and reached a consensus.
Before implementation, over 20 cases were rated by the four researchers trained on the
scale as pilot cases; after inter-rater reliability was reached, the actual rating began. Throughout
the process, previously rated cases were re-evaluated to ensure that the researchers were
consistently rating cases over time.

54
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

(iii) Determining What Information to Rate
The Police Foundation scale was designed to rate cases based on all the information and
evidence gathered by the police in their files during an investigation. Our project, however,
involved information from several different sources, including police but also prosecutors and
defense attorneys, and were not ongoing investigations but rather cases that had progressed to a
certain point (i.e., conviction, plea, acquittal, or dismissal) at the time that innocence was
determined. Therefore, it was necessary to clarify what sources of information would be taken
into consideration when rating each case.
In rating the state’s case for the erroneous convictions, researchers assessed all the
evidence the state had gathered or reasonably should have gathered up through trial or the plea.
All state actors (including prosecutors, police, and state crime analysts) were considered to have
exchanged information and evidence, so anything known to one group of actors was imputed to
the state’s case. Therefore, withholding evidence, making intentionally false statements, and
similar actions by a state actor were considered “known” by the state. Mistakes or false
statements by non-state actors that were unknowable by the state were not included in our
assessment of the state’s case. The aim was to ensure that we considered only that evidence
which was accessible to the trier of fact (judge or jury) or the prosecutor (who made the decision
to pursue the case) and therefore could have conceivably contributed to the erroneous conviction.
In rating the defense’s case for the erroneous convictions, if the case went to trial,
researchers assessed the evidence the defense presented at trial. If the case resulted in a plea,
researchers assessed all exculpatory evidence the defense gathered and shared with the state up
through the plea. Because the defense’s case, unlike the state’s, generally involved only one or

55
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

two attorneys and a very small number of assistants, there was generally no need to impute
knowledge between actors when rating the defense.
In rating the state’s case for the dismissals, researchers assessed all the evidence the state
gathered or should have gathered up to the point immediately before the discovery of the
exonerating evidence that directly led to the dismissal. This included any exculpatory evidence
uncovered by the state before the final piece of exonerating evidence was discovered. We
excluded the exculpatory evidence that directly led to the dismissal of the case because the goal
was to rate the strength of the information that led to the erroneous indictment in a manner that
paralleled the way the erroneous convictions were rated. That is, we considered the evidence
leading to the erroneous conviction but not information leading to the eventual exoneration.10
Again, as with the erroneous convictions, all state actors were considered to have exchanged
evidence, so anything known to one group of actors was imputed to the state’s case.
In rating the defense’s case for the dismissals, researchers assessed all the evidence the
defense gathered up to the point of dismissal, excluding exculpatory evidence uncovered solely
by the state. Finally, in rating the defense’s case for acquittals, researchers used the same
standard as that employed for the defense of erroneous convictions that went to trial.

                                                            
10

If we had included the exonerating piece or pieces of evidence in the dismissals but not in the convictions, the
strength of evidence rating system would be virtually meaningless—clearly, the dismissals would be consistently
much weaker than the convictions because they would include conclusive evidence of innocence. The purpose of the
rating scale was to determine whether, before the exonerating piece of evidence was known to the state, there was
already a difference in strength between the near misses and the erroneous convictions that might speak to why the
cases ended up one way and not the other.

56
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

III. QUANTITATIVE ANALYSIS

Having collected such detailed information about the erroneous convictions and near
misses, the project’s next task was to compare the relative frequencies of these variables between
the two sets of cases.11 To better organize the analyses, we divided the variables into six
conceptual categories that may predict an erroneous conviction versus a near miss. They include:
(1) location effects, (2) nature of the victim, (3) nature of the defendant, (4) facts available to the
police and prosecutor, (5) quality of work by the criminal justice system, and (6) quality of work
by the defense.
III.A. Frequencies and Bivariate Analysis
III.A.1. Statistically Significant Variables
Using bivariate analysis, we tested each of these variables for significance against case
outcome (erroneous conviction or near miss). Tables 3-8 show the results by category.

[Tables 3-8 about here]

Because many of the cases in our sample were crimes of murder or sexual assault (or a
combination thereof), we re-ran the bivariate analyses controlling for each type of crime. We
also controlled for time period. Our concern was that enhanced forensic technology and
improved police procedures might skew the distribution of erroneous convictions and near
                                                            
11

The reader will note that some of the variables in Appendix VIII.A.do not speak directly to the different outcomes
of erroneous conviction and near miss cases. Such variables, including the amount of compensation that a defendant
was awarded after exoneration, the length of a defendant’s prison sentence, and the mechanisms by which
defendants were exonerated, were recorded for possible future research and are not analyzed in our current study;
therefore, we do not discuss them further in the report.

57
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

misses over time, and we wanted a control variable that reflected the point at which forensic
methods and investigative techniques would have significantly advanced over prior periods
(Table 9-10). We selected 1989 for these purposes, which represents the first year that DNA was
used to exonerate an innocent defendant. Finally, we controlled for one state—Illinois—in
which erroneous convictions were disproportionately located. Here, our concern was that the
added frequency may have reflected the circumstances under which erroneous convictions were
uncovered as opposed to those forces that would have led to a mistaken conviction.12 In this
vein, we checked the case distributions for disproportionate influence from other localized
“scandals,” such as problems in the Houston crime lab or corruption in the Los Angeles Police
Department’s Rampart Division. Those effects were not sufficiently sizeable to necessitate a
control. Indeed, as we discovered in the regression analysis, the disproportionate number of
cases from Texas was controlled for by other independent variables, including state punitive
culture.
In applying the controls noted above, we only used those in which there was a theoretical
reason to expect that it may have affected that variable. For instance, there was no reason to
think that cases from Illinois were more likely than cases from other states to present DNA
evidence, so we did not control this variable for state. By contrast, given the prevalence of
organic material suitable for DNA testing in rapes rather than murders or robberies, we
controlled the DNA variable for type of crime.
As shown in Tables 11-22, when controlled for type of crime, time period, or state, many
of the variables did not remain statistically significant. With few exceptions, noted below, the
variables that remained significant despite the controls were retained as candidates for logistic
                                                            
12

Illinois has seen considerable attention by innocence projects and journalists to uncover erroneous convictions,
especially in capital cases. The findings were so profound that they led former Illinois Governor George Ryan to
commute the sentences of all defendants then on the state’s death row.

58
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

regression. Furthermore, a number of variables that washed out when controlled were
nevertheless also retained for further testing due to their original significance when all cases
were analyzed or their theoretical importance in prior criminal justice research; those, primarily,
were variables representing race effects.13

[Tables 11-22 about here]

At the same time, despite the fact that they remained statistically significant when
controlled, a few variables had such low frequencies that they violated the assumptions of the chi
square tests and could not be included in the regression models. These included: the presence of
medical error, whether federal law enforcement participated in the case investigation, and
whether the defense presented DNA evidence. In addition, notwithstanding its statistical
significance, we did not consider it necessary to retain the U.S. census region where the case
occurred because inter-state differences were accounted for by such variables as presidential
vote/citizen ideology14, death penalty culture (which served as a measure of state punitiveness),15
and state crime rate, among others.
Table 23 shows the variables that remained (by category of hypothesis) for further testing
in logistic regression.
                                                            
13

Interestingly, as shown later, those added variables did not prove explanatory in logistic regression.
We originally tested mean presidential vote by state for the years at issue (see Appendix VIII.E., Statistical
Coding Book). As an alternative and more diverse measure, we then evaluated citizen ideology. This variable was
defined as the average citizen ideology score from 1980 until 2008 (most recent data available). The citizen
ideology score was developed by Berry et al. (1998) and measures roll call voting scores of state congressional
delegations, the outcomes of congressional elections, the partisan division of state legislatures, the party of the
governor, and various assumptions regarding voters and state political elites.
15
We measured the death penalty/state punitiveness variable in several ways, including: (a) state executions post1976 per state population, (b) state executions post-1976 per number of state murders, (c) state death sentences post1976 per number of state murders, (d) county death sentences post-1976 per number of county murders as calculated
by Liebman et al. (2002), and (e) a ranked level of state punitiveness developed by Kutateladze (2009). We
primarily used the first measure in bivariate analysis, but as noted in Section III.B.2., the second measure proved to
be more robust in the logit regression models.
14

59
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Table 23 about here]

These variables included:






















State Death Penalty Culture (Executions per Population)
Crime Rate Consistency between Year of Crime and 5 Years Prior
Age of Defendant
Criminal History of Defendant
Defendant’s Race (% African American)16
Defendant Graduated High School
White Female Victim
Female Victim
High Profile Case
Strength of Prosecution’s Case
Error in Forensic Evidence Presented by Prosecution
Non-Eyewitness Testimony or Evidence17
Discrepancy in Victim’s Description of Defendant (Testimony Discrepancy)
Intentional Misidentification by Eyewitness
Prosecution Withheld Evidence
Time from Arrest to Indictment
Strength of Defense
Defendant Offered Physical Alibi
Defendant Offered Other Suspect
Defendant Offered Evidence of Official Misconduct
Defendant Offered Family Witness

III.A.2. Variables That Do Not Distinguish the Cases
Perhaps one of the most important findings from this project is which variables do not
explain the different outcomes between erroneous convictions and near misses. Indeed, a
number of variables that are often discussed by scholars as possible sources or “causes” of
                                                            
16

We also tested an alternative variable to evaluate the influence of race, calculated as the percentage of the state
population in 1990 that was non-white (all racial minorities plus Hispanics). This variable was designed in particular
to measure "racial threat" -- the concept that states become more penal as the size of the minority population rises
(and whites, thereby, feel more threatened).
17
Except where specifically indicated, we use “testimony” in this report as a short-hand to indicate testimony or
evidence that is given to the state. Because the majority of the near misses did not go to trial, for most variables it
was necessary to compare trial testimony in the erroneous convictions with evidence given by witnesses in
anticipation of trial in the near misses. As discussed in V.C., we recognize that this type of comparison has inherent
limitations.

60
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

erroneous convictions were not correlated with case outcome in the bivariate analysis. Among
these are eyewitness misidentification, false confessions, and jailhouse informants.
This is not to say, however, that these variables are unrelated to either erroneous
convictions or near misses. To the contrary, such factors as eyewitness misidentification and
false confessions occurred regularly in both sets of cases, but those rates were so similar that
they do not explain divergent case outcomes in the bivariate analysis. We address this point in
greater detail in the Discussion section, arguing that these errors may lead to the indictment of an
innocent defendant. But, once a factually innocent defendant enters the criminal justice system,
the findings here suggest that conviction will not turn on differences in those factors.
(i) Errors in Eyewitness Identification
Most of the variables reflecting errors in eyewitness identification were statistically
indistinguishable between erroneous convictions and near misses, and of those that were
significant when all cases were analyzed, with the notable exception of the intentional
misidentification variable (discussed below), virtually all washed out when controlled for type of
crime. Eyewitness identification error was frequent in both sets of cases (83 percent of
erroneous convictions and 75 percent of near misses), and the difference was not significant.
Other eyewitness variables that did not prove significant included: the number of eyewitnesses,
whether the eyewitness was certain of her identification, whether the eyewitness provided a
description of the defendant, whether the victim needed multiple tries to identify the defendant,
whether the non-victim eyewitness made a cross-racial identification, and the type of
identification made by the non-victim eyewitness (e.g., photo array, show-up).

61
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

(ii) False Confessions
When examined across multiple categories of the variable, 22 percent of erroneous
convictions involved defendants who either falsely confessed to a crime or provided
incriminating statements against themselves. In near misses, these categories numbered 29
percent of the cases. Those differences were not statistically significant. Examining confessions
only, however, 14 percent of erroneous convictions involved false confessions compared to 22
percent of near misses; these data were statistically significant. Nevertheless, when false
confessions were controlled separately for type of crime and date, the differences between cases
rapidly dropped to statistical insignificance. As a result, we did not include the variable in later
regressions.
Among cases where the defendant falsely confessed, less than one third of the erroneous
convictions and the near misses involved a substantiated allegation of physical threat or abuse to
elicit the confession. This means that the majority of false confessions were either volunteered
or obtained through purely psychological coercion.
(iii) Official Error and Misconduct
Neither error nor misconduct (excepting Brady violations) on the part of prosecutors and
police were significantly different between the two sets of cases. Error was defined as an
inadvertent mistake or omission (such as forgetting to collect or losing evidence), while
misconduct often violated the defendant’s constitutional rights and involved an element of
intentionality or extreme negligence (including coercing a confession or planting physical
evidence). Although the overall frequencies of all types of error or misconduct were low, police
error occurred most frequently, appearing in 16 percent of erroneous convictions and 11 percent
of near misses. Prosecutorial error was less common and occurred in extremely similar

62
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

percentages (six-seven percent) in both cases. Police misconduct occurred in eight percent of
erroneous convictions and 12 percent of near misses, whereas prosecutor misconduct occurred in
only three-four percent of both sets of cases. For each variable, we also recorded error or
misconduct that was alleged, but this was not statistically significant (see Table 6).
Fraud by a forensic scientist, e.g., when a forensic scientist knowingly reported or
testified to false results, was also infrequent and not significant in bivariate analysis. Among
erroneous convictions, six percent of cases proved fraud and the same percent alleged it, whereas
among near misses it was four percent for each. Therefore, while forensic fraud was a serious
issue in a few of our cases, it is not endemic in the system. In fact, instances of fraud were
generally confined to a handful of specific labs and technicians.
(iv) Criminal History
Although a defendant’s prior conviction was significant in the bivariate analysis, among
defendants with a prior conviction, there was not a significant difference in the number of the
prior convictions or whether the priors involved a history similar to the crime at hand. In fact, in
both sets of cases only a relatively small percentage of defendants had a record that was similar
to the crime at issue (17 percent among erroneous convictions, 25 percent among near misses).
Our results suggest that it is simply prior involvement with the criminal justice system that puts a
suspect at greater risk for an erroneous conviction, rather than the specifics of that involvement.
(v) Jailhouse Informant
Finally, despite the fact that false statements by a jailhouse informant or snitch have been
noted as a “cause” of erroneous convictions in prior literature (Gould & Leo, 2010; Gross, 2005),
such intentional falsehoods by a snitch were relatively infrequent in our dataset, occurring in 11
percent of erroneous convictions and seven percent of near misses. This difference was not

63
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

significant. However, jailhouse informants and snitches were included in a variable that was
significant in regression analysis—lying by any non-eyewitness—so we cannot discount that this
type of evidence might play some role in distinguishing erroneous convictions and near misses.
(vi) Other Factors
In both sets of cases, very few defendants were a member of a gang: just four percent
among erroneous convictions and seven percent among near misses. This suggests that the
stigma of gang membership does not substantially hurt the innocent defendant.18 Finally, neither
the erroneous convictions nor the near misses made frequent use of defense experts. Seventeen
percent of erroneous convictions included a defense expert, whereas 22 percent of near misses
did; the difference was not statistically significant. Thus, any overall difference in the strength
of the defense between the two types of cases is probably not attributable to the use of experts.
III.B. Logistic Regressions
III.B.1. Estimating the Models
To test the impacts of the variables that were significant in bivariate analysis on case
outcome, we created a series of logistic regression models estimating the probability of an
erroneous conviction.19 As with the bivariate analysis, we controlled for time period (post
DNA), type of crime (murder), and state (Illinois).20 To recover missing data, we used multiple
imputation (a technique discussed below) and, in turn, generated five separate datasets. Our
estimations then incorporated information from all of the data. Each model in Tables 24-28

                                                            
18

This is true only for the crimes at issue here—predominately rape, murder, and robbery. The influence of gang
membership on erroneous indictments or convictions involving drug crimes may be different.
19
Logit or probit models will both produce similar coefficient estimates that are different by a factor of about 1.7
(Long & Freese, 2006).
20
Although here we report the models controlled for murder, we estimated all of the models with rape instead of
murder and got similar results.

64
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

provides the number of observations (460 complete cases) per dataset (5 datasets) for a total of
2,300 observations.

[Tables 24-28 about here]

Most statistical models assume that the errors across observations are homoscedastic
(e.g., they do not vary based on the value of the independent variables) and independent. In our
model, we had individuals that were clustered in states. In so far as there may be some
correlation among individuals in the same state, the cases are probably not independent and
likely violate the assumption of homoscedasticity. To address this issue, we implemented a
clustering correction by clustering the standard errors by state where the case resides.
Tables 24-28 show simple models with control variables and a single conceptual focus:
the nature of the crime (Table 24),21 the nature of the defendant (Table 25) , the nature of the
facts (Table 26), the quality of the work by the criminal justice system (Table 27), and the
quality of the defense (Table 28). Table 29 then provides the results of the full model with all
components of each model as well as the relavant control variables. In this report we focus on
the discussion of the final full model (Table 29), but we provide these other models to show that
the results are similar with just a small set of variables and not sensitive to the specification.
Before we discuss the results of the full model in more detail, however, we must first mention
our choices to use multiple imputation for missing data and the Receiver-Operating
Characteristic Curve as an evaluation of goodness-of-fit.

                                                            
21

The nature of the crime model combines the variables that were in two separate conceptual categories in the
bivariate analysis—location effects and nature of the victim.

65
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Imputation is the process of filling missing data with plausible values. The primary
benefit of multiple imputation is retention of case data; because of this, as King, Honaker,
Joseph, and Scheve (2001) note, methodologists and statisticians are nearly unanimous in their
agreement that multiple imputation is a better technique to deal with missing data than the
conventional applied data analysis approach—listwise deletion. We therefore used multiple
imputation on our dataset when confronted with missing values. For a more detailed explanation
of imputation, see Appendix VIII.I.
To evaluate the model fit in our binary dependent variable models we chose to use the
Operating Characteristic (ROC) curve, which improves upon other measures of goodness-of-fit,
such as the percent correctly predicted (PcP) and the percent reduction in error (PRE). (For a
more detailed explanation of the ROC curve and alternatives, see Appendix VIII.I.) The area
under the ROC curve and above the 45 degree line gives a unique measure of model fit. In the
models that we estimate, we use this area under the ROC curve statistic as the measure of model
fit. While there is not a standard for what this number should be, between 80-89 percent is
generally considered a good model, and above 90 percent is an excellent model at predicting the
outcome of interest.22 As explained later, the area under the ROC curve for our regression is
90.8 percent, suggesting a strong predictive model.
III.B.2. Regression Results
Table 29 displays the results from our full model of logistic regression. The first column
organizes the variables by conceptual category. The second column shows the name of the
individual indicator, and the third and fourth columns include the coefficient and standard error.

                                                            
22

See http://gim.unmc.edu/dxtests/roc3.htm and http://www.cpdm.ufpr.br/documentos/ROC.pdf for discussions of
this rule of thumb.

66
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

These standard errors are both adjusted based on the inclusion of five datasets as well as
clustered by state.
[Table 29 about here]

Among the variables related to the nature of the crime and location effect, only the death
penalty culture/state punitiveness measure23 has a consistent impact on case outcome—
defendants in states with greater use of the death penalty are more likely to be erroneously
convicted. Other such variables, including the demographics of the victim, are insignificant.
Among defendant characteristics, the defendant’s age and any prior criminal history
influence case outcome. Older defendants are less likely to be erroneously convicted, whereas
defendants with prior criminal histories are more likely to face an erroneous conviction. Other
defendant characteristics, such as race and high school graduation, have no impact in
distinguishing between an erroneous conviction and near miss.
Three factors related to the nature of the facts are associated with erroneous convictions,
although not necessarily as expected. To be sure, errors in forensic analysis increase the
likelihood of an erroneous conviction, but the stronger the prosecution’s case, the less likely that
a defendant will be erroneously convicted and instead will see his case dismissed or acquitted.
So, too, intentional misidentification is associated with a decrease in the likelihood of an
erroneous conviction. The other three factors are insignificant.
Evaluating the work of the criminal justice system, two variables are associated with an
increased probability of a erroneous conviction. The prosecution’s withholding of evidence and

                                                            
23

As noted in footnote 15, we used several alternative measures of state punitiveness. State death penalty culture,
defined as the number of executions post 1976 per number of murders, proved to be the most robust measure in the
regression analyses, and thus we select it for the final model.

67
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

lying by a non-eyewitness24 are both positively associated with a erroneous conviction. The
time to arrest from indictment is not related.
Three components of the quality of defense are associated with a change in the
probability of a erroneous conviction. When the defense has a stronger case, a erroneous
conviction is less likely. Additionally, when the defense presents evidence of misconduct, it
reduces the likelihood of a erroneous conviction but at a lower degree of certainty than the other
measures (p<0.10). Having a family witness is associated with an increase in the likelihood of a
erroneous conviction. Other evidence presented by the defense, including a physical alibi and
another suspect, do not systematically relate to case outcome.
Finally, all of the control variables—Illinois cases, the Post-DNA period, and murder
cases—are associated with erroneous convictions. Although we do not discuss the substantive
influence of these variables, their inclusion assures us that the results for the key theoretical
variables are not spurious. As seen in Figure 3, the area under the ROC curve is 90.8 percent,
suggesting a strong predictive model. The more simple models have ROC curves in the 80s,
suggesting good predictions (see Figure 2), but the more complete model offers the most
accurate predictions and thus the best fitting model.

[Figures 2-3 about here]

Since the logit coefficients are difficult to interpret directly, we produced a graph of the
predicted probabilities for each variable that has a statistically significant impact on the
                                                            
24

While this variable was not significant in bivariate analysis, it was included in the regression models because prior
research provides both theoretical and empirical evidence that it plays a substantial role in erroneous convictions
(see Section I.B.4). An included variable, jailhouse informant or snitch, was also tested in the regressions but was
not significant.

68
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

likelihood of an erroneous conviction. Figure 4 displays these results. Using the statistical
program Clarify (King, Tomz, & Wittenberg, 2000), we performed 1000 simulations over the
five datasets and predicted the change in the probability of an erroneous conviction given
changes in the independent variables while holding all other variables at their means. Using
Boehmke’s (2008) algorithm, we then plotted these predicted probabilities and their standard
errors. For the dichotomous variables (Prior Criminal History, Forensic Evidence Error,
Intentional Misidentification, Intentionally False Non-Eyewitness Testimony or Evidence,
Strength of the Prosecution’s Case, Prosecutor Withheld Evidence, Strength of the Defense’s
Case, and Family Witness), we show the change in the probability of an erroneous conviction
given a change in one of these variables from 0 to 1. For the continuous measures that are
statistically significant (Death Penalty Culture and Age of the Defendant), we display the change
in the probability of an erroneous conviction given a change in the independent variable from its
minimum to its maximum.

[Figure 4 about here]

As Figure 4 shows, an increase in the Death Penalty Culture measure from its minimum
(0.000) to its maximum (0.012) is, on average, associated with a 27 percent increase in the
probability of an erroneous conviction.25 As with any estimate, there is uncertainty around it, but
the result is clearly positive and likely substantively meaningful. Similarly, as the age of the

                                                            
25

This 27 percent increase is a 27 percentage point increase. In other words, if the baseline probability for a
wrongful conviction were 56 percent, then this change in Death Penalty Culture would be associated with an 83
percent chance of a wrongful conviction. The statistical software Clarify produces first differences, or the expected
percentage point change in the dependent variable given a change in the independent variable of interest.

69
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

defendant increases from its minimum (14) to its maximum (76), the likelihood of an erroneous
conviction decreases, on average, by 65 percent.
A strong defense case decreases the probability of an erroneous conviction by an average
of 24 percent. Conversely, a strong case by the prosecutor decreases the probability of an
erroneous conviction by an average of 25 percent. If the prosecution withholds evidence or a
non-eyewitness gives intentionally false evidence, the probability of an erroneous conviction
increases by 19 percent and 16 percent respectively. A forensic evidence error is associated with
a 14 percent increase in the likelihood of an erroneous conviction. An intentional
misidentification of a suspect leads, on average, to a 20 percent reduction in the probability of an
erroneous conviction. If a family member is called as a witness by the defense, the probability of
an erroneous conviction increases by 13 percent on average. Finally, a defendant’s prior
criminal history increases the probability of being erroneously convicted by an average of 19
percent.
III.B.3. Prediction/Forecasting
As the ROC curves show, the statistical model is able to predict erroneous convictions at
a high rate (0.908). Nearly 91 percent of the time the model can accurately predict an erroneous
conviction versus a near miss. Even in the simpler models (Tables 24-28), the area under the
ROC curve is between 0.801 and 0.827. While this evidence suggests that the models are useful,
there is a concern with overfitting—that is, the fit we find may be particular to these data. The
model may be good at predicting outcomes in the same data used to estimate the model (insample prediction), but we do not know how the predictive ability of the model will apply to
other datasets (out-of-sample prediction).

70
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

To address the concern of overfitting, we used the procedure of cross-validation to create
an out-of-sample test of the model using our own data set (see Appendix VIII.I.) for technical
discussion of cross-validation). The results of cross-validation show that our model is able to
predict over 87 percent of the cases of erroneous convictions out of sample. This is a slight
decrease as compared to the in-sample forecasts (91 percent) but the forecasts are still high,
suggesting overfitting is not a serious problem with the model. We expect that this model could
be used to predict future cases across the United States that are likely to be erroneous convictions
with a high degree of accuracy.

71
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

IV. QUALITATIVE ANALYSIS AND THE EXPERT PANEL
IV.A. Convening the Expert Panel
A vital component of the project was to supplement the quantitative analysis with an
independent qualitative review of our cases by an expert panel, which met over two days in early
February 2012 at the American University campus in Washington, DC.
The expert panel consisted of twelve criminal justice professionals. Panelists included:
two prosecutors, two retired judges, a defense attorney, a police sergeant, a forensic scientist, and
researchers on both police and prosecutor practices. We selected the panelists with the goal of
providing a diversity of opinions and experiences. Thus, we selected panelists who held
sometimes opposing positions in the adversarial system, from different jurisdictions across the
United States, and reflecting a mixture of academic scholars and practitioners. The names of
potential panelists were obtained either through nominations from other experts or by their
reputation as experts in their respective fields.
To facilitate the panel’s assessment, the project created factual narratives for 39 of our
cases: 20 erroneous convictions and 19 rightful dismissals/acquittals. We matched these cases
according to the Police Foundation rating scale for both the strength of the prosecution’s case
and strength of the defense (see Appendix VIII.F for exemplars from the scale). Thus, there
were examples of weak, probative, and highly probative prosecution cases and weak, probative,
and highly probative defenses among both the erroneous conviction and near miss narratives.
These narratives included both DNA and non-DNA exonerations as well as representative fact
patterns (e.g., false rape claims, sudden infant death) and the most common felonies (rape,
murder, and armed robbery). This allowed us to facilitate an efficient and broadly applicable
discussion among the experts. Appendix VIII.G contains six sample narratives; note, though,
72
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

that we have omitted any identifying information in the narratives or our discussion of case facts
in this report, which is in compliance with our human subjects research protection plan. The
panelists, however, had full access to the information on each case.
The panelists were invited to consider the case narratives several weeks prior to the panel
and were asked to assess each case using an evaluation sheet provided by the project (see
Appendix VIII.H). The evaluation sheet asked the panelists, among other things, to weigh the
strength of various types of evidence against the defendant (such as a confession or eyewitness
identification), consider the errors that led to the erroneous indictment or conviction, and offer
suggestions for concrete steps or practices that could have been taken to prevent these errors.
The panelists then brought these evaluation sheets with them to the panel discussions as a way of
facilitating their memory of the cases and providing the project with written commentary.
At the panel, the experts were invited to discuss the reasonableness of individuals’
actions in the cases. The project was looking for a qualitative analysis of why the various
participants in these cases may have acted as they did. The panelists were also encouraged to
discuss what, if anything, differentiated the cases that led to an erroneous conviction from those
that resulted in a dismissal or acquittal. Finally, the experts assisted by recommending
appropriate measures that might enhance the ability of the criminal justice system to identify and
appropriately respond to factually innocent defendants. The discussions were moderated by the
principal investigator, who encouraged the experts to cover a broad range of topics and speak
freely. As a result, the panelists appeared to be remarkably candid in their opinions and the flow
of conversation was dynamic. One project staff member was in charge of taking notes
throughout the two days of conversation. These notes, along with the evaluation sheets from the

73
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

individual panelists, were reviewed at the close of the expert panel and formed the basis of the
project’s qualitative analysis.
IV.B. Qualitative Analysis
The expert panelists’ analysis helped develop and further explain the individual factors
that were statistically significant in distinguishing the two sets of cases. Even more importantly,
the panel placed the quantitative findings within the greater context of the criminal justice
system, creating a narrative of the system-wide processes that can influence case outcome.
IV.B.1. Predictor #1: Prior Convictions
Quantitative analysis indicates that having at least one prior conviction is a predictor of
an erroneous conviction but does not further explain the nature of this connection. To
understand more fully how a prior record may hurt an innocent defendant, we turned to the
panelists. To begin, the expert panel noted that prior convictions often influence the decision of
police officers to place a suspect in a lineup. In addition, if the defendant has a previous arrest or
conviction, his picture is likely to be in a mug book; that fact alone may place a defendant in
jeopardy that a witness will erroneously choose his photo for a crime he did not commit.
However, this explanation is insufficient to explain the quantitative results, because both
erroneous convictions and near misses involved similar rates of eyewitness identification errors.
According to the panel discussion, something else must be at work. If an innocent defendant is
mistakenly identified by an eyewitness (or implicated in a crime in some other way, such as an
anonymous tip), the police and prosecutors must reach a decision—is the defendant a viable
suspect or is the evidence against him misplaced? The answer to this question helps dictate the
progression of the subsequent investigation, because in the first instance they will likely be
working to rule in the defendant whereas in the second instance they will be more likely to
74
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

exclude him. According to the panelists, a defendant’s criminal history may play an important
role in whether officials view the defendant as a viable suspect. If, for example, the defendant is
an upstanding member of the community, the police are more likely to view inculpatory
evidence with skepticism, thinking, perhaps, that this is not the sort of person who is likely to
commit a crime, and are more likely to investigate whether a mistake has been made. The
opposite would be true if the defendant presented a significant criminal example.
Our cases provide a good example of this phenomenon. In one case the panelists
discussed, a child rape victim tentatively identified the defendant in a lineup. He was a young
man with several unrelated prior convictions. Despite the fact that the victim was never more
than 60 percent sure of her identification, the police pursued the matter against the defendant and
worked to build a case against him. Eventually, the state obtained a conviction based on weak
circumstantial evidence and microscopic hair comparison. In this case, the defendant’s criminal
history not only influenced the police’s decision to put him in a lineup, it also preconditioned the
police and prosecutors to take the identification as conclusive evidence of his guilt—despite the
fact that the victim was a child and the identification tentative.
Put another way, if the defendant had been a model citizen, the officials would have been
more likely to view the identification as too weak to pursue a conviction or at the very least,
would have attempted to exclude the defendant as a suspect by looking for exculpatory evidence.
Instead, although the defendant’s prior convictions were not at all similar to rape, they were
enough to make him a plausible suspect that the police and prosecutors were determined to
retain. They continued looking for some inculpatory evidence until they found enough to
convince a jury. Thus, while some of the panelists noted that considering a defendant’s prior
history is not always a mistake, and in fact may be important in the early stages of an

75
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

investigation, this case illustrates that it is vital to limit the consideration to only similar crimes
and to avoid letting prior history cloud judgments about the strength of evidence against the
defendant in the case at hand.
IV.B.2. Predictor #2: Intentional Misidentification
Misidentifications as a whole did not differ appreciably between erroneous convictions
and near misses, nor did most variables about the identification process (e.g., length of time from
crime to identification, cross-racial identification, and level of certainty). However, when we
distinguished intentional misidentifications from honestly mistaken identifications, the difference
became statistically significant, with honest mistakes predicting erroneous convictions and
intentional misidentifications associated with near misses. Although it may seem counterintuitive, a close reading of the cases revealed that a lying eyewitness may actually be easier for
police and prosecutors to detect with further investigation than one who is honestly mistaken.
For example, in one near miss in our study, the “victim” was a college student who said she had
been raped by her professor. While her identification of the professor was credible, further
police investigation uncovered strong indications that she was lying (emails that she had
retouched, a forged restraining order, etc). When confronted with this evidence, the victim
confessed that she had made up the sexual assault. By contrast, if the case had involved an
honest misidentification by the victim (as did the majority of erroneous convictions), there would
probably not be a “smoking gun” for the police to find and discredit the identification.
IV.B.3. Predictor #3: Forensic Errors
Forensic evidence contributed to the exoneration or exclusion of the majority of cases in
this study, but earlier errors in science also led to many of the false indictments or convictions.
Indeed, as the quantitative results revealed, forensic errors helped predict erroneous convictions.
76
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

What sort of errors were these and how did they occur? According to the panelists’ close
reading of the cases, the most common forensic error was improper testimony at trial by a state’s
witness who overstated the precision or inculpatory nature of the results she obtained. For
instance in a case involving microscopic hair analysis, the scientist erroneously stated that there
was a 1 in a 10,000 chance that the defendant’s hair was not the hair found on the rape victim’s
bed. This was highly misleading because no reliable statistics exist to analyze hair matches.
Instances in which an analyst incorrectly or fraudulently performed a test, planted evidence, or
changed reports were much rarer. This is confirmed by the statistical analysis, which shows that
forensic fraud was relatively uncommon (see Section III.A.2) and that errors in serology were far
more likely to stem from testimony than testing.
Poor communication between the forensic lab and the police and prosecutor’s offices, as
well as inadequate training among criminal justice officials, also contributed to the erroneous
convictions. In some cases, the police did not adequately identify or preserve forensic evidence,
in part because they were unaware of the evidence’s potential significance. For example, in one
case the panelists discussed, a jewelry store robber brought a mailed package to the scene as a
decoy. After the crime, this box was placed in evidence and tested for latent fingerprints, which
did not match the defendant. However, because the investigating agency that collected the box
and conducted the testing was not the same agency that led the subsequent investigation, the
exculpatory results were not passed on to the prosecution or revealed at trial.
In this case, a serious breakdown of communication between the agencies meant that
exonerating forensic evidence, though properly tested, was not presented until many years after
the defendant’s conviction. Although the cause of this communication gap is unclear, it appears

77
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

that adequate safeguards, such as evidence checklists or a pre-trial forensic review, were not in
place at the time.
Finally, discussions with the panelists revealed that errors with forensic evidence and
testimony often can be linked to the problem of tunnel vision. Rather than using forensic testing
early in the investigation to better understand the crime and evaluate potential suspects,
investigators frequently turned to testing later in a case to confirm the police or prosecutor’s
belief in the defendant’s guilt and ensure a conviction. This may explain why in some cases
there was a serious delay in testing the physical material, often right up until the time of trial. If
the forensic evidence were seen as largely playing a confirmatory role in the investigation, there
was naturally less incentive to obtain results as soon as possible.
IV.B.4. Predictor #4: Weak Prosecution Case
In logistic regression analysis, weak case facts (or weak prosecutions) predicted
erroneous convictions, while the near misses generally had stronger prosecution facts. This was
a counterintuitive finding, since we might suspect that the cases with weaker evidence against
the defendant would be more likely to end in a dismissal or acquittal. One possible explanation
for this finding is that it is an artifact of our requirement that every case must have an official
recognition of innocence. It seems possible that when a case has gone to trial and resulted in a
conviction, prosecutors and other officials might only be forced to acknowledge the defendant’s
innocence when the case is weak; otherwise, the time investment and public commitment to the
case may persuade officials to continue to proclaim the defendant’s guilt. By contrast, in a near
miss, prosecutors and police may not have invested as much in the case and are thus more
willing to recognize innocence even when the case originally appeared strong. While this

78
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

explanation might account for some of the variance, it is unsatisfactory for two reasons.26 First,
a number of our near misses actually involved serious commitment by prosecutors and police,
including months of investigation (often with the defendant in jail), public press conferences, and
considerable resources expended. In addition, it appears more likely that an official recognition
of innocence is influenced by the strength of evidence of innocence after conviction, rather than
the original strength of the case. That is, prosecutors and other officials are probably forced to
recognize the defendant’s innocence in cases where the evidence of innocence is virtually
incontrovertible, regardless of whether the other evidence such as eyewitness identification or a
confession appeared strong at the time.
The panel discussion of the cases offered an alternative explanation for the relationship
between erroneous convictions and weak prosecution cases. A noticeable portion of our cases
started with a tip or hunch that implicated the defendant in the crime. Often the tip came from an
anonymous caller or paid informant, while the hunches usually originated with a police officer
who knew the defendant from a previous run-in with the law. While tips and hunches are not
worthless, they are generally very weak evidence. They are open to abuse from the real
perpetrator or a rival who dislikes the defendant; in addition, hunches often rely on vague
similarities between the defendant and the description of the suspect. Indeed, it was common in
our cases to have the defendant placed in a lineup based on a witness’s vague or general
description of the suspect (e.g., black male in his 20s-30s). Once the defendant was identified by
the witness, however, the hunch suddenly metamorphosed into a “case,” even though the basis
for submitting the defendant to jeopardy in the first place was remarkably flimsy.

                                                            
26

In addition, we want to emphasize the many of our cases did not involve a prosecutor’s recognition of innocence,
but instead relied on a juror, a judge, or a governor.

79
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Though police may rightfully have felt obliged to consider a tip or hunch, often this weak
evidence led investigators to narrow the hunt prematurely. Rather than remaining in a general
stage of investigation, where police consider a broad range of evidence and leads, the
investigation turned into an active pursuit of a particular defendant. Convinced of the
defendant’s guilt despite a lack of conclusive proof, the prosecutor may enlist a snitch to provide
corroborating evidence. Or, a prosecutor may become so attached to a case that he fails to
recognize that the available evidence is not only weak but also exculpatory. This happened in
another case in our sample:
Case A: While walking with his wife and child, a man was attacked and killed in a park
by a group of teenagers. Police asked a neighborhood boy to come to the station for a
lineup because he knew someone the victim had gotten into a fight with earlier; the boy’s
friend tagged along to the station and agreed to be placed in a lineup too. Both boys were
identified by the victim’s family. Though one eyewitness was a child and the other, the
wife, had been tainted earlier by seeing a picture of one of the defendants and being told
his name, the case against the two defendants went to trial based on their identifications.
No forensic evidence was recovered. At trial, it came out that the wife had been drinking
prior to the crime and gave inconsistent testimony about what she saw in the melee that
night. In addition, the prosecutor successfully moved to exclude the confession of
another man and did not correct the wife’s perjury on the stand when asked if she had
prior crimes (in fact, she had pending drug charges and warrant out for her arrest). Both
defendants were convicted.
In Case A, the original basis of suspicion against the defendants was quite weak, and the
identifications were also problematic. To sustain the case, the prosecution excluded useful
information about a confession and withheld the wife’s prior history. Unfortunately, the
cumulative effect of multiple pieces of doubtful evidence was still an unreliable case. In
addition, in some ways this type of case was particularly difficult to defend against. There was
no physical evidence to test, and the defense had little it could do but attack the reliability of the
witnesses. Thus, when the prosecution’s case is weak, the possibility increases that an error will
go unchecked.

80
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

IV.B.5. Predictor #5: Weak Defense Case
In the quantitative analysis, the strength of the defense case helped to predict case
outcome, with erroneous convictions having on average weaker defenses than near misses. From
our discussion with the expert panel, it emerged that a large problem with defense work in the
erroneous convictions was a lack of time, training, and funding among the defense bar.
Conflicts of interest also weakened the defense in some cases. The defense attorney had
competing loyalties to other suspects or was working too closely with local prosecutors. This
was most likely to be a problem in small communities where the pool of defense attorneys is
limited and the same prosecutors and defense attorneys work together almost every day. One of
our cases epitomized the danger of multiple conflicts and poor training.
Case B: Several young adults living in a small town were co-indicted for murder. At
least two of the defendants had histories of mental illness and had been in therapy with a
local psychologist before the crime. However, the psychologist was also a deputy sheriff,
who interviewed the defendants about the murder and suggested that they were repressing
memories of the crime. In doing so, the psychologist not only violated professional
standards, he also became unavailable to testify for the defense. In the same case, the
defense attorney for one of the defendants suggested to his client what might have
occurred at the victim’s apartment. His suggestions, which mirrored the story of the
police, became the blueprint for his client’s false confession. The confession, along with
confessions from the defendants who saw the psychologist, was used against a codefendant. The defense never hired an outside expert to examine the defendants’ mental
conditions.
The defense in this case suffered from a multitude of flaws—conflict of interest, no
independent expert witness, lack of training regarding false confessions, belief in the defendants’
guilt—all of which conspired to create a hopeless situation where the defendants felt compelled
to take pleas for a murder they did not commit.
By contrast, panelists noted that several dismissals involved a defense attorney who took
considerable initiative to prove the defendant’s innocence. In one example, because the
prosecution failed to investigate the defendant’s alibi, the defense attorney tracked down
81
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

witnesses and work records from the defendant’s seasonal job. He was then able to create a
detailed timeline of the defendant’s drive home that night, including producing clerks and
receipts to verify each of the stops along the way. In another case involving several juvenile
defendants, defense counsel convinced the judge to discard a large portion of his clients’
confessions, in part because he hired one of the preeminent experts on false confessions. The
attorneys in these cases successfully forced the state to reconsider seemingly inculpatory
evidence and were instrumental in preventing an erroneous indictment from turning into a major
miscarriage of justice.
In the discussion among the panelists, however, it emerged that in some cases the
strength of the defense did not rest on the quality of counsel. A number of erroneous convictions
appeared to have a weak defense because there was simply little or no exculpatory evidence
available. For instance, if the defendant were unemployed or a drug addict, defense counsel
often found it impossible to establish an alibi or produce reliable witnesses to testify to the
defendant’s character and whereabouts. In addition, many of the cases involved a stranger rape
at night in the victim’s home. Not surprisingly, most defendants had no alibi for this time except
a wife or girlfriend, who at best could testify that she would have heard if the defendant had left
the bed that night. Such a defense naturally did not successfully counteract the victim’s
identification of the defendant as the rapist. Interestingly, this could explain why, in the
regression analysis, presenting a family member as a defense witness predicted an erroneous
conviction. Therefore, a weak defense usually stemmed from either poor defense counsel or a
lack of exculpatory evidence—or both.

82
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

IV.B.6. The Role of the Judge
Finally, although judicial error did not emerge as a significant issue in the quantitative
analysis, our discussion of the cases suggest that it may play some role. Judges, like defense
attorneys, appeared to lack training and education in new advances in psychology, forensic
science, and other related disciplines. More importantly, in a number of our cases, the judge
failed to use his or her discretionary powers to closely examine the evidence, level the field
between prosecution and defense, or otherwise take an active role in protecting the innocent
defendant:
Case C: A defendant was charged with robbery. A key component of the case against
him included statements from a confidential informant. The judge allowed a government
official to testify in court about the informant’s implication of the defendant, rather than
requiring that the informant appear in court for cross-examination or holding an in
camera review of the informant’s veracity. The defendant was convicted. During a
subsequent investigation by the state, the informant was tracked down; he declared that
he never spoke with the official about the defendant. When confronted, the official
changed his story to say that a confidential contact of the confidential informant gave him
the information—by then, it became clear that the source never existed.
In Case C, although the judge was not responsible for the government official’s perjury, had he
adequately examined the testimony before or during trial, the exculpatory evidence would likely
have been revealed; such evidence would have led to a dismissal, or at the very least, a much
stronger defense. In other cases, the judicial error more directly contributed to the erroneous
conviction:
Case D: A young, Hispanic gang member was charged with armed robbery and carjacking. When the car was recovered, the latent fingerprints and DNA found on the
clothes left by the bandit within the vehicle failed to match the defendant. Based on
eyewitness identifications from a three-person photo array and a show-up, the defendant
was offered a plea of two years in prison. Rather than rejecting the plea outright, or at
least ensuring that the defendant understood the nature of the deal, the judge threatened
him with life imprisonment until he took the plea mid-trial.

83
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

The judge in Case D was clearly convinced of the defendant’s guilt, despite compelling evidence
to the contrary. This clouded his judgment regarding the appropriateness of a plea and allowed
the prosecution to obtain a conviction even though the defendant would likely have been
acquitted.
What these cases have in common is not so much legally recognizable judicial error
(again, our quantitative results suggest such error is rare), but instead a scenario in which
mistakes made earlier by police, prosecutors, eyewitnesses, or defense attorneys were
compounded when judges failed to perform their gate-keeper function to prevent injustices.
Thus, judges are part of a more comprehensive breakdown of the adversarial system, and when
the system fails, erroneous convictions can happen. In the section below, we explore the concept
of system failure in greater detail.
IV.B.7. How Factors Interact: Tunnel Vision
If we synthesize our qualitative analysis of the individual factors that distinguish the two
sets of cases, it emerges that miscarriages of justice are complex break-downs in the adversarial
process, that occur when errors are compounded rather than rectified. Taking an overarching,
system-wide approach allows us to examine the interactive processes and pathways that facilitate
breakdown and influence the outcome of a case.
Most of our cases involved more than one error, sometimes as many as four or five. This
was particularly evident with the erroneous convictions. For example, false testimony or
evidence alone was rarely the direct cause of an erroneous conviction. The larger story often
involved a prosecutor who had serious doubts about the witness’s story but did not share these
doubts with a superior or the defense, and a defense attorney who did not have the time or energy
to investigate the witness’s story. Thus, our research more readily describes a “perfect storm” of
84
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

system failure in the adversarial system that differentiates erroneous convictions from near
misses. As such, it is not entirely correct to say that the witness’s falsehood “caused” the
erroneous conviction; instead, it is only part of the story.
Our statistical analysis does not speak to the order of these errors that lead to conviction.
However, a qualitative look at the cases shows that the errors are often sequential and build upon
each other. Panelists commented that many of the near misses and erroneous convictions started
in a similar way—most frequently a misidentification, but occasionally a tip or confession—that
led to an indictment. This explains the quantitative findings, that indicated the cases generally
possessed these problems at similar rates.
The panelists’ analysis also reiterates that such factors alone cannot “cause” an erroneous
conviction. If they did, all of our cases would have ended in a conviction. Instead, the very
different case outcomes stemmed from what occurred after the initial error or misconduct.
Among the erroneous convictions, early mistakes were usually compounded with additional
errors or lack of attention, such as improper or extremely weak forensic evidence and a defense
attorney who either did not bother to prepare an adequate defense or lacked the training and
money to do so. Case A, above, provides an example of two eyewitness identification errors that
were later compounded by prosecutorial misconduct.
By contrast, among the near misses, the original errors were corrected in a variety of
ways. These included: better or more complete forensic testing, an active defense attorney who
tracked down and documented an alibi, or a follow-up investigation in which the victim or
witness recanted the identification. For example, one near miss involved a severely drugaddicted husband, found at the scene of the crime, who admitted he could have killed his
prostitute wife in a drug-induced haze. Despite the fact that the husband was indicted as the

85
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

“perfect” suspect, the police ordered blood tests on physical evidence near the victim—and when
the tests came back to exclude the husband, the police took the results seriously and ordered a
fuller investigation. After painstakingly tracking another suspect from the victim’s hotel room to
a local hospital, the state eventually built a much more solid case against the other suspect, who
then confessed. In this case, a thorough and unprejudiced police investigation uncovered the
weaknesses in the initial inculpatory evidence against the defendant and saved him from a
conviction.
What accounts for this difference in the way the cases proceeded? Why were errors
rectified in some cases and compounded in others? The panelists offered several explanations.
At times a lack of effective institutional safeguards may have allowed errors to go undetected or
uncorrected. Such safeguards include evidence checklists, proper file maintenance, open
communication between officers and supervisors, rigorous screening of cases by seasoned
prosecutors, and adequate defense funding. These practices could have gone a long way in
steering an erroneous indictment away from a conviction. But according to the panelists, what
was more fundamentally at issue in most erroneous convictions, what further separated these
cases from the near misses, was tunnel vision.
Though tunnel vision has been listed as a cause of erroneous convictions in prior
literature (Gould & Leo, 2010; Findley & Scott, 2006), our panelists helped elucidate the concept
in terms of the more heavily theorized process in psychology and management of escalation of
commitment (Brockner, 1992; Coleman, 2010; Staw, 1981). As more resources—money, time,
and emotions—are placed into a narrative involving a suspect, the actors involved are less
willing or able to process negative feedback that refutes their conclusions. Instead, actors want
to devote additional resources in order to recoup their original investment. As a result, evidence

86
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

that points away from a suspect is ignored or devalued, and latent errors are overlooked. At this
point, the police are working to rule in rather than rule out the suspect, and prosecutors have
moved from “inspection” mode to “selling” mode. Escalation of commitment contributes and
facilitates system breakdown because it dismantles the rigorous testing of evidence that makes
the adversarial process function effectively.
To a large extent, the panelists attributed tunnel vision in our cases to a police and
prosecutorial culture in which questioning and independent thinking were not valued, procedures
were not designed to probe already gathered evidence, and little or no concern was given to
learning from past errors. Even if safeguards, such as those mentioned above, are in place, they
cannot be used effectively when the officials in the system are blinded by tunnel vision.
Community pressure or concerns about community safety also contribute to escalation
and tunnel vision. In several cases, especially those involving sex crimes against children or the
murder of police officers, investigators were under additional pressure to make an arrest and take
a dangerous and likely repeat felon off the streets. While officers may be originally justified in
arresting the defendant based on weak suspicions in order to ensure public safety, often the state
placed too much stock in the arrest and turned a blind eye to the potential weaknesses or flaws in
the case. High profile arrests may also receive greater attention and support from supervisors
and politicians, making it more difficult for officers or prosecutors to let the suspect go even if
they want to. One final case illustrates how the entire process worked to convict an innocent
defendant:
Case E: A young white female was raped and killed in a large city park—the second
recent female rape victim in the area. The defendant was a suspect because he had
recently been convicted of purse-snatching in the park. The only evidence against him at
the time of arrest, however, was a positive hair comparison performed by an FBI analyst.
To bolster the case, the prosecutor ordered serology testing of the semen found in the
victim and also enlisted the help of a paid informant. The informant was engaged to
87
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

speak to the defendant in prison and elicit a statement. The serology results excluded the
defendant. At trial, the state explained away the serology by saying the sample had
degraded and relied instead on the informant’s statement and the hair comparison to
obtain a conviction.
This case had multiple problems, including community pressure to solve the crime,
prejudice against the defendant because of his prior crime, forensic fraud (the FBI analyst’s hair
results were actually spurious), and a paid informant. Yet our qualitative analysis throws into
question the accepted wisdom that any of these caused the erroneous conviction, because none of
the issues functioned by itself. For instance, if the prosecutor, after being told about the hair
comparison, had reinvestigated the case for more probative indications of guilt rather than
providing a paid informant with a “road map” of the type of statement he wanted, the results
would very likely have been different. Similarly, even if the prosecutor had used the paid
informant but the serology results had been taken seriously, the defendant probably would not
have been convicted.
If we think in terms of “conditional causation” or “pathways analysis,” it becomes clear
that this case, like erroneous convictions in general, is more understandable as a systemic failure
than as the malfunction of any given part. Our expectation of the criminal justice system is not
to be perfect—human errors will always occur—but rather to rigorously test the evidence of
guilt. When this does not happen, the system, rather than revealing mistakes and untruths, can
serve to mask, compound, and even legitimize these errors.
In a concluding section we discuss our findings in more detail and introduce possible
remedies to mitigate or prevent compounded error and system-wide breakdown. We also note
the limitations of this study and offer suggestions for future areas of research.

88
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

V. DISCUSSION

As the preceding discussion indicates, there are both narrow and broad ways of
interpreting the study’s results. If one follows a narrow approach—asking only if the hypotheses
proffered distinguish erroneous convictions from near misses—the results highlight a number of
key variables that influence how innocent defendants are convicted instead of cleared. Foremost
among these are the strength of the evidence offered and the defense provided, although here the
results do not necessarily match our original hypotheses. Certainly, the quality of defense is
related to erroneous convictions, with weaker defenses linked to conviction. But, contrary to
expectations, cases with weaker facts for the prosecution were more likely to end in an erroneous
conviction than dismissal or acquittal.
A state’s capital culture proved relevant in distinguishing cases—a conclusion we
anticipated—as defendants charged in jurisdictions with a stronger attachment to the death
penalty had a greater chance of erroneous conviction than those prosecuted elsewhere. Also
influential were a defendant’s age and prior record, with younger defendants and those
previously convicted of any crime at heightened risk of erroneous conviction. These results were
expected, although we had also anticipated that erroneous convictions would be tied to a
defendant’s prior history of similar criminal activity, which they were not. Examining case facts,
we were not surprised to find that erroneous convictions were connected to inadvertent
misidentification or lying by a non-eyewitness, but we had not expected that testimony by a
family witness would prove influential. Finally, as hypothesized, Brady violations, forensic
error, and tunnel vision proved influential in distinguishing erroneous convictions from near
misses.

89
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Even if many of these results were anticipated, perhaps the more interesting results are
those hypotheses that were not borne out by the research. For example, among the traditional
legal model of wrongful convictions, the study provided support for only half of the original
hypotheses. Although the findings distinguished erroneous convictions and near misses on the
basis of tunnel vision, Brady violations, forensic error, and inadvertent misidentification, the
disparate case outcomes were not explained by other eyewitness variables, false confessions,
forensic fraud, or non-Brady error or misconduct by police or prosecutors. Similarly, our
findings support just one-third of the hypotheses under the sociological factors model.
Explanations focused on the race of the defendant or victim, the socioeconomic standing of the
defendant, the political culture or crime rate of the surrounding state, or media attention to the
crime were not statistically significant. Finally, the results fail to confirm a hypothesis focused
on the time between crime and arrest. Table 33 provides a summary of these conclusions.

[Table 33 about here]

V.A. Broader Interpretation
It might be tempting to rely on the narrow interpretation of the study’s results and
conclude, perhaps provocatively, that some traditional explanations for wrongful convictions—
most notably, prosecutorial misconduct and false confessions—do not explain the erroneous
convictions here. One might even claim these presumed sources are not responsible for
wrongful convictions. That, however, would be wrong. It is essential to recall what
distinguishes the two sets of cases in this study. In both categories, an innocent person enters the
criminal justice system by indictment or information. In one set, the defendant’s prosecution
90
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

continues to an erroneous conviction; in the other, the defendant is acquitted or sees his case
dismissed. So, when our results exclude a potential variable or hypothesis, the correct
conclusion is that this factor does not explain the trajectory of an erroneous conviction once an
indictment has issued. Or, stated differently, the factors identified in this study offer
explanations for why one group of factually innocent defendants is convicted and the other
released after an initial error has been made in indicting them.
How a factually innocent defendant enters the criminal justice system is another matter.
Here, our study helps to tell a more complex story. Although we lack a comparison group of
accurate convictions, it is impossible to ignore the large number of both erroneous convictions
and near misses that contain eyewitness errors. As described earlier in Table 7, more than threequarters of the cases involved a misidentification (stemming from either an honest mistake or an
intentional false implication). Theoretically, it is possible that this proportion might be as high
among accurate conviction cases, but we think it unlikely. Not only is the rate so excessive as to
seem unusual, but misidentification is a substantial error at the bedrock of a prosecution—the
kind of mistake that, if caught, would later lead to dismissal or exoneration.
The same logic applies to false confessions and police and prosecutor misconduct, each
of which was found at relatively similar rates in both the erroneous conviction and near miss
cases. Although their respective rates are not as high as the misidentifications in our sample, the
likelihood that a false confession or official misconduct would lead to an erroneous conviction,
let alone indictment, is substantial. As prior research on wrongful convictions has shown, as
many as half of known exonerations may involve at least one of these errors (Gould, 2007).
Looking at our findings through a broader lens, then, the key distinction between the
erroneous convictions and near misses is the point in the criminal justice system at which they

91
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

differ. We know that both sets of cases share similarly high rates of misidentification, false
confession, and official misconduct and that, further, several of these factors are not statistically
significant in explaining the ultimate resolution of innocence cases. Our supposition is that there
is a difference in what contributes to the indictment of an innocent defendant and what factors
then prevent a mistaken indictment from leading to an erroneous conviction. This process is laid
out in Figure 7 below.

[Figure 7 about here]

Based on prior wrongful conviction research, we surmise that false confession, official
misconduct and some sort of identification (either an anonymous tip or a misidentification) are
among the errors that bring innocent defendants into the criminal justice system. That is,
regardless of the ultimate resolution of a case, an innocent individual is more likely to be
indicted for a crime if there is a misidentification or official misconduct in his case or if he is
induced to confess falsely. But official error and the like need not necessarily lead to an
erroneous conviction, for there are several factors that intervene in the investigation and
prosecution of a crime that then influence the likelihood of a mistaken conviction. Most
significantly would be the quality of defense provided to a defendant; here, one can imagine how
a defendant who was incorrectly identified as the perpetrator could still be cleared of the crime
by an intrepid defense attorney and investigator, who conduct thorough legwork to poke holes in
the state’s case. As discussed above, this is more likely to occur when the misidentification is
intentional rather than inadvertent. So, too, it is understandable how a younger defendant might
not have the sophistication to aid in his defense or the credibility to have his alibi fully

92
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

investigated by detectives. We can imagine as well why defendants charged in states with a
heightened capital culture would face police and prosecutors more committed to the original
theory of the case, even if it later appeared to have holes; by contrast, individuals in less punitive
jurisdictions might benefit from a law enforcement community willing to consider exculpatory
theories or evidence. For that matter, the escalation of commitment by police and prosecutors in
more punitive jurisdictions could well contribute to the failure of prosecutors to turn over
exculpatory evidence, a factor identified in the erroneous conviction cases.
The one finding in Figure 7 that does not make intuitive sense is the inverse relationship
between the strength of the prosecution’s evidence and the likelihood of an erroneous conviction.
As discussed previously, our findings indicate that innocent defendants are at a heightened risk
of mistaken conviction when the facts available to the prosecution are weaker. We had
originally hypothesized the opposite based on the assumption that police and prosecutors would
be able to weed through the facts of indicted cases and move to dismiss those with the weaker
facts.
As described earlier, we view this result in a larger context of system failure. That is,
what primarily distinguishes the erroneous conviction cases from near misses is that the criminal
justice system largely failed the former once a defendant had been indicted. Regardless of what
brought innocent defendants into the system, the officials charged with protecting their rights
more often failed those who were erroneously convicted than those who had their cases
dismissed or were acquitted. The erroneously convicted had weaker defenses than other
innocent defendants; their prosecutors were less willing to turn over exculpatory evidence when
required by law; their cases relied disproportionately on flawed forensics and lying noneyewitnesses; and their investigators more often engaged in tunnel vision, perhaps because

93
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

criminal justice officials were under heightened pressure by their local culture to punish a
presumed wrongdoer or because police officers figured the defendant must have done something
wrong because he was young and had a prior record. Under these circumstances, it is
understandable how a case would proceed to judgment even though it had weaker prosecution
facts than did those cases the state dismissed. Indeed, if there is but one conclusion from our
research it is that, overall, the erroneously convicted are truly cases of systemic failure. Just as a
jetliner may crash when a multitude of problems arises and distracts the crew’s attention from
the task at hand (National Transportation Safety Board [NTSB], 1973),27 erroneous convictions
see a combination of errors by those charged with control of the criminal justice system;
unfortunately, this “perfect storm” leads to systematic injustice.

V.B. Recommendations for Reform
In presenting these findings to select audiences in anticipation of public release, we often
have been asked which problems uncovered in this study deserve the most attention. “So, you’re
saying that erroneous identifications and false confessions aren’t the real problem, and that more
attention should be directed to criminal defense and other issues post-indictment?” more than
one person has asked us. Our short answer is no, but, of course, that hardly explains the issue.
Our findings indicate that there are problems in the criminal justice system that both lead to the
indictment of the innocent and also prevent the dismissal or acquittal of innocent defendants
once they enter the criminal justice system. The question for criminal justice policymakers, then,
is where in the process to focus attention. If the issue is what leads to the indictment of the
innocent, then our findings point to mistaken identifications as well as false confessions and
                                                            
27

The 1972 crash of an Eastern Airlines airplane into the Florida Everglades was ascribed to the crew’s inattention
to the autopilot, which had been deactivated while the pilots were troubleshooting the malfunctioning of its landing
gear (NTSB, 1973).

94
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

official misconduct. If, instead, one seeks to correct an erroneous indictment once it has
occurred, then the focus should be on criminal defense practice, forensic evidence, and
prosecutorial discretion, among other issues.
Another way to think about our findings is to distinguish between dynamic and static
sources of erroneous convictions. Here a static source is a case fact presented to, but not created
by, criminal justice professionals. A good example in our dataset is a defendant’s age. When
confronting static sources, police, prosecutors, and defense lawyers would do well to keep watch,
knowing that such facts may put a defendant at a greater risk for erroneous conviction even while
recognizing that they did not create the source themselves. By contrast, dynamic sources are
those in which the actions or omissions of criminal justice officials heighten the risk of an
erroneous conviction. A classic example here would be Brady violations, where the failure of
police and prosecutors more directly affect the outcome of a case.
Looking at our data this way, the vast majority of sources we identified are dynamic –
thus presenting the greatest opportunity for criminal justice professionals to ameliorate the risk
of erroneous conviction themselves. The strength of the prosecution’s case and the quality of
defense lawyering, of course, are dependent upon the actions of criminal justice officials, as are
forensic fraud, Brady violations, and tunnel vision. These are all areas in which heightened
professionalism and greater attention to casework can reduce the chance of an erroneous
conviction. But even misidentification – whether inadvertent by an eyewitness or intentional by
a non-eyewitness – rests to some extent on the judgment and decisions of police and prosecutors.
Not that we expect either would suborn perjury, but a trier of fact would only be aware of such
evidence if the state chooses to offer the testimony. As such, both offices would do well to better
screen the motivation and veracity of witnesses, their opportunity for identification, and the

95
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

reasonableness of their testimony before presenting testimony that may raise the risk of an
erroneous conviction.
This is not to say that criminal justice professionals are “off the hook” with respect to
static sources. To be sure, a police officer or prosecutor cannot affect a defendant’s age or
criminal history, nor is a defense lawyer responsible for the capital culture of the state in which a
case is brought. But, simply being aware of the influence of these factors may cause all three
groups to pause and give greater attention to cases that present a combination of risks. When
representing a young defendant in a particularly punitive state, for example, a defense lawyer
should be especially careful to ensure that even she does not get caught up in tunnel vision,
assuming that she grasps the theory of the case before investigating a variety of alternatives.
Similarly, police ought to be on guard that they do not “rule in” a suspect simply because he has
a criminal history, especially when the prior crime does not match the case at hand.
How, exactly, the criminal justice system can prevent erroneous convictions is a complex
matter. We believe the findings here will help in two important respects – first by distinguishing
between those factors that bring innocent defendants into the criminal justice system and those
sources that then heighten the risk of conviction post-indictment. As we have said, we do not
seek to prioritize one issue over the other but instead to note that the sources – and thus
responses – to each are different. Second, we think it helpful to distinguish between those
sources directly under the control of criminal justice professionals and those influences that
confront the criminal justice system through secondary means. Both sets of sources require
attention, but, again, they entail different approaches.
Apart from this more broad brush response to erroneous convictions, the project
benefitted from the participation of expert panelists eager to go beyond the sources of erroneous

96
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

convictions and offer recommendations that would prevent their occurrence. Several of these
measures have been presented in other publications (see Gould, 2007), but because the
recommendations were offered by such a professionally varied group, and in candid and
constructive dialog, we include the expert panelists’ recommendations here as well. We endorse
their recommendations as a step forward in addressing the sources of erroneous conviction
uncovered by our research, both those problems that lead to the indictment of innocent
defendants and those that hinder the adversarial system from clearing innocent individuals
already charged with a crime. Of note, we offer them here not as a proven set of answers but
rather as the product of serious discussion among dedicated and talented criminal justice
professionals, with the hope that their presentation may spur additional discussion and
consideration.

(i) Defense Practice


There is no antidote more powerful than a committed attorney with sufficient time and
resources to mount an adequate defense. In this respect, increasing caseloads and
declining resources for indigent defense present a heightened risk of erroneous
convictions.



It is essential that a defendant meet with his attorneys as soon as possible after arrest or
charge so he can participate in his defense and the lawyer has a full opportunity to
investigate facts before indictment or at least trial.



Just as prosecutors must disclose exculpatory evidence, defense attorneys must request
and then review evidence offered in discovery.

97
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

(ii) Disclosing Exculpatory Evidence


Prosecutors should pursue a policy of open discovery; the practice might include holding
discovery conferences, in particular with forensic evidence presented, so there is no
question whether Brady material was offered. This approach would benefit the
prosecution as much as the defense, since withholding would be more difficult to argue
on appeal.

(iii) Eyewitness Misidentification


Because the trial process has consistently failed to weed out bad identifications,
safeguards are necessary before this point in a case; prosecutors, in particular, need to be
more aware of how important the indictment stage is.



There should be a threshold of evidence met before police officers place someone in a
lineup. The standard need not be unduly high, simply an articulated reason that might
even be shared with the defense. If this is not practical, police departments should at
least foster a “mindfulness” among officers so that they do not put defendants in jeopardy
of a misidentification without good reason.



Officers should build a photo array or lineup based on the description given, rather than
on the suspect’s appearance. This way the suspect’s appearance does not become the
“baseline” upon which a comparison is made.



If the police do not have a description, they should rely on a mug book for initial
identifications. This is less prejudicial than creating a lineup or photo array based on the
suspect’s appearance, which may or may not be similar to what the actual perpetrator
looks like.

98
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.



Show-ups can be replaced with quickly assembled photo arrays using new computer
software; these new photo arrays combine the immediacy of show-ups with the less
prejudicial nature of a photo array. So far, computerized arrays have been used in only a
few jurisdictions, and officers must be trained on this method to make it efficient.

(iv) False Confessions


Videotaping interrogations (and even interviews) should become standard practice;
videotaping provides an important safeguard for defendant and police alike.

(v) Forensic Error


Forensic investigation—especially DNA testing—should be conducted early in a case to
rule suspects in or out rather than confirming or checking what police and prosecutors
already believe once an investigation gains steam.



More jurisdictions should have police officers trained in crime scene investigations who
could be present when technicians are collecting evidence to help direct the collection
and expand the area searched.



Prosecutors need to be better educated in the techniques of forensic testing and must
clarify what results mean if they do not understand a report.



There should be better supervision and peer review in forensic labs, including the kind of
“grand rounds” or discussion of near misses that the medical community follows in a
similar context when conducting morbidity and mortality conferences. (See a similar
point below about systematic failings.)

99
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

(vi) Police Misconduct


To help combat erroneous convictions based on police corruption, jurisdictions might
allow defense counsel to request the police officers’ police record. (Such practices would
also strengthen the quality of defense provided defendants.)

(vii) Systematic Failures


Police and prosecutors should develop checklists of items to weigh when investigating or
charging a case. These might include forensic reports, the circumstances of interrogation,
and the consideration of alibis. In particular, alibis offered—or reviewed—at the last
minute are less likely to be credited by police and prosecutors even if they are truthful.
For this reason, it is important that alibis be investigated before the state commits
considerable time and resources to a case.



The criminal justice system could embrace the medical model of professional review,
which sees errors as a cultural and systemic problem rather than the “bad apple”
approach.



Departments and agencies should hold a review when there is an erroneous conviction or
a near miss. The process should be routine and involve everyone, along the lines of
“grand rounds” conducted by doctors.



To encourage reporting and facilitate discussion, departments might consider
immunization (at least internally) of officers who accurately report errors or near misses.

100
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

(viii) Tunnel Vision


Police and prosecutors need a “devil’s advocate” in their midst to consider alternative
hypotheses of a case. Whether this should be located in one office or the other, it is
essential that police officers and prosecutors remember to pause and not rush to
judgment.



Police departments might invite the stationing of a prosecutor, whose job it would be to
review evidence as it is collected “at the source” before an indictment issues.

(ix) Weak Prosecution Evidence


Prosecutors’ offices should consider assigning more experienced prosecutors to charging.
This would fulfill the “screening” function better and save resources devoted to cases that
later must be dismissed.



Prosecutors should consider the standard they use for indicting a defendant. Although
some prosecutors use a standard of likelihood of conviction at trial, others employ a
much looser standard to hold a defendant while additional evidence is collected. If a
weaker standard is used, prosecutors should deliberately evaluate (or reevaluate) the
merits of the case at regular intervals to avoid the trap of escalation of commitment.

V.C. Study Limitations
As the first large-scale empirical study of erroneous convictions using a control group,
our research is primarily exploratory and possesses certain limitations. Like all studies of
erroneous conviction, we could not draw randomly from a known universe of cases. Although
we searched widely for both erroneous convictions and near misses, the data are limited by our
ability to have uncovered possible cases. In addition, while we identified the two sets of cases
101
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

using similar methods, we were more successful in locating potential erroneous convictions
through lists and databases already created by scholars and activists. By contrast, there has been
no systematic collection of near misses; therefore, we had to rely more on case solicitation and
media searches to locate these cases.
This difference in method of case selection may affect our dataset. Relying on media
coverage of a case may oversample murders, which are the most serious and therefore most high
profile crimes in the community. Case solicitation likely also oversamples more recent cases,
since the individual who recommended the case to us is most likely to remember relatively
recent cases that she was involved in or heard about. In fact, our near misses did include more
murders and more recent cases than the erroneous convictions. We addressed these twin issues
by controlling for type and date of crime, a process we employed in both the bivariate and
regression analyses. For that matter, we are uncertain whether the issue of case selection can be
avoided in future research. In time, scholars may begin to compile a list of near misses, but we
suspect that given the relative frequency and low profile of near misses as opposed to
exonerations of the erroneously convicted, there will never be parity between the ways used to
identify the two sets of cases.
More troubling for our purposes is the possibility we oversampled near misses that
involved false confessions. One of the authors has written extensively on false confession cases,
including those that resulted in a dismissal or acquittal; because of our familiarity with such
cases and our method of case selection, we may have included more of these cases than would
have occurred with a truly random sample. This, in turn, could influence our finding that there
was not a statistically significant difference between the rate of false confessions in erroneous
convictions and near misses. At the same time, we may have oversampled erroneous convictions

102
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

from states with the death penalty because of the increased visibility of these exonerations (see
footnote 6), leading us to find what may be a spurious relationship between state punitiveness
and erroneous conviction. Further research on near misses is needed to clarify whether our
findings on these two variables are affected by any artifacts of collection bias.
The near misses were also limited in the type and extent of data available. An inherent
limitation in our study is that the different procedural postures of the cases may artificially
depress the observation of certain errors or misconduct in the near misses. For example, because
the near misses generally did not involve appeals, post-conviction investigations, or habeas
proceedings like the erroneous convictions, it may be less common for prosecutorial misconduct
to be uncovered.28 Likewise, the vast majority of the dismissals had no transcripts and court
documents. For many of these cases we had to rely on news reports or the memory of witnesses
for the same information that was obtained through transcripts for many of the erroneous
convictions. Recognizing that such disparity introduces a possible imbalance in the
completeness of information between the erroneous convictions and near misses on a few
variables, such as number of defense witnesses or length of time between the crime and the
identification, we did not make these variables central to our analysis.
Another limitation, which is shared by all researchers in the field of erroneous
convictions, is that the cases studied are likely not representative of the majority of criminal or
even felony cases. As Garrett (2008), Gross (2005), and others have pointed out, most known
erroneous convictions and exonerations are for serious violent crimes, such as murder, rape, and
robbery. In part this is because DNA evidence, the most powerful and uncontroverted method of
proving innocence, is most often present in these types of crimes, whereas it is not available or
                                                            
28

It should be noted, however, that a fair number of the near misses involved civil proceedings for compensation
involving wrongful arrest, etc., and these proceedings often provided an official recognition of error or misconduct
on the part of police or prosecutors.

103
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

analyzed in lesser offenses. In addition, the serious punishment for such crimes leads to greater
incentives to prove an erroneous conviction (Garrett, 2008: 19, Gross, 2005). Furthermore,
dismissals or acquittals for less serious crimes usually do not receive enough system scrutiny or
media attention to make them readily identifiable. Even among rapes and murders, the known
erroneous convictions and near misses are likely over-representative of cases with DNA
evidence (Gross & O’Brien, 2008: 939; Gross, 2005). Therefore, while our focus on serious
violent felonies is probably inevitable and does not create a bias between the erroneous
convictions and near misses, it should be kept in mind that the manner in which the criminal
justice system identifies and handles innocent defendants accused of lesser or different crimes
may be significantly different than our analyses suggest.
Finally, a word must be said about the use of imputation. Despite our considerable
efforts, there were some data in particular cases that proved impossible to find, for example a
defendant’s educational level among the near misses. Rather than engage in listwise deletion of
these cases, we employed the statistical technique of imputation. Appendix VIII.I provides a
detailed explanation of imputation, which is a mainstream method to deal with missing data.
Although we would have preferred to avoid any missing data, our limited use of imputation
permits analysis across a greater number of cases.
V.D. Possible Directions for Future Research
In our study, the control group was near misses, meaning that we are able to show how
the criminal justice system identifies and corrects the indictment of innocent defendants before
an erroneous conviction occurs. There are other control groups that could be used in the future –
for example “rightly” convicted defendants. Similarly, future research could look at different
decision points in the criminal justice process, such as the prosecution’s decision to indict. None
104
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

of this weakens our work or the significance of our findings. Rather, it emphasizes the context
of our research and underscores that our findings are not so much about what causes erroneous
convictions but what prevents them once an innocent defendant enters the criminal justice
system. Our control group was the appropriate choice for this research question, but other
equally interesting questions will require a different control group.
Further research into near misses is also a promising avenue for development. As the
first systematic study of these cases, our project may raise as many questions as it answers.
Future studies that examine near misses will be needed to verify whether our results apply to
other contexts. For instance, researchers could examine a much narrower group of near misses,
such as those involving drug crimes or false confessions, which would provide a more nuanced
view of the major issues we explored in this project. To build understanding of how the criminal
justice system identifies and deals with innocent suspects in the majority of cases, future projects
could also identify near misses that involve less serious felonies or misdemeanors. Because of
their relative frequency, near misses may actually have greater potential than erroneous
convictions to shed light on this area of the system. To study cases involving lesser or different
crimes, researchers will come up against some of the data collection problems we noted earlier.
To overcome these limitations, researchers may profit from building a working relationship with
police and prosecutors in jurisdictions where officials are open to interviews or willing to share
police records.
Lastly, our research helps inaugurate a new era of social science research on erroneous
convictions. Such research moves beyond anecdotal stories and the delineation of fixed
“causes,” focusing instead on the more dynamic and complex question of what these cases can
tell us about how the criminal justice system safeguards, or fails to safeguard, innocent

105
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

defendants who have been erroneously accused. Our project shows that such system-wide,
large-scaled comparison studies can and will bear fruit.

106
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

VI. REFERENCES

Acker, J.R., Brewer, T., & Cunningham, E. (2001). No appeal from the grave: Innocence,
Capital Punishment, and the lessons of history. In S.D. Westervelt & J.A. Humphrey
(Eds.), Wrongly convicted: Perspectives on failed justice (pp. 154-174). New Brunswick,
NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Aldrich, J., & Nelson, F. (1984). Linear probability, logit, and probit models. Beverly Hills,
CA: Sage University Press.
American Bar Association. (2006). Achieving justice: Freeing the innocent, convicting the
guilty. In P. Giannelli & M. Raeder (Eds.), Report of the ABA Criminal Justice Section’s
Ad Hoc Committee to Ensure the Integrity of the Criminal Process. The American Bar
Association.
Baldus, D., Woodworth, G., & Pulaski, C. (1990) Equal justice and the death penalty: A legal
and empirical analysis. Boston, MA: Northeastern University Press.
Baumgartner, F.R., De Boef, S., & Boydstun, A. (2008). The decline of the death penalty and
the discovery of innocence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
Beck, N., Epstein, D., Jackman, S., & O’Halloran, S. (2001). Alternative models of dynamics
in binary time-series-cross-section: The example of state failure. Paper presented at
the 2001 Annual Meeting of the Society for Political Methodology. Emory University,
Atlanta, GA, 1-38.
Beck, N., King, G. & Zeng, L. (2000). Improving quantitative studies of international conflict: A
conjecture. American Political Science Review, 94(1), 21–36.

107
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Bedau, H.A., & Radelet, M.L. (1987). Miscarriages of justice in potentially capital cases.
Stanford Law Review, 40(1), 21-179.
Bedau, H.A., & Radelet, M.L. (1988). The myth of infallibility: A reply to Markman and Cassell.
Stanford Law Review, 41, 161-170.
Bedau, H.A., & Radelet, M.L. (2004). Convicting the innocent in capital cases: Criteria,
evidence, and inference. Drake Law Review, 52, 587-603.
Behrman, B.W., & Davey, S.L. (2001). Eyewitness identification in actual criminal case: An
archival analysis. Law and Human Behavior, 25, 475-491.
Bernhard, A. (2001). Effective assistance of counsel. In S.D. Westervelt & J.A. Humphrey
(Eds.), Wrongly convicted: Perspectives on failed justice (pp. 220-240). New Brunswick,
NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Berry, S.M. (2003). Bad lawyering: How defense attorneys help convict the innocent. Northern
Kentucky Law Review, 30, 487-503.
Berry, W.D., Ringquist, E.J., Fording, R.C., & Hanson, R.L. (1998). Measuring citizen and
government ideology in the American States, 1960-93. American Journal of Political
Science, 42 (1), 327-348.
Blandon-Gitlin, I., Sperry, K., & Leo, R.A. (2011). Jurors believe interrogation tactics are not
likely to elicit false confessions: will expert witness testimony inform them otherwise?
Psychology, Crime & Law, 17(3), 239-260.
Boehmke, F. (2008). Plotfds: A Stata utility for plotting first differences. Iowa City, Iowa.
Borchard, E. (1932). Convicting the innocent: Sixty-five actual errors of criminal justice. Garden
City, NY: Garden City Publishing Company, Inc.

108
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Brockner, J. (1992). The escalation of commitment to a failing course of action: Toward
theoretical progress. The Academy of Management Review, 17(1), 39-63.
Clarke, J.W. (1998). Without fear or shame: Lynching, capital punishment and the subculture
of violence in the American South. British Journal of Political Science, 28(2), 269289.
Cochran, J., Paquette Boots, D., & Chamlin, M. (2006). Political identity and support for capital
punishment: A test of attribution theory. Journal of Crime and Justice, 29(1), 45-79.
Cohen, S. (2003). The wrong men: America’s epidemic of wrongful Death Row convictions. New
York, NY: Carroll & Graf Publishers.
Cole, G.F., & Smith, C.E. (2010). The American system of criminal justice (Ed.12). Belmont,
CA: Wadsworth Publishing.
Cole, S.A. (2001). Suspect identities: A history of fingerprinting and criminal identification.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Coleman, M.D. (2010). Sunk costs and the commitment to medical treatment. Current
Psychology, 29: 121-134.
Connery, D. (1996). Convicting the innocent: The story of a murder, a false confession and the
struggle to free a wronged man. Northampton, MA: Brookline Books, Inc.
Daubert v. Merrell Dow Pharm. Inc., 509 US 579 (1993).
Davis, D., & Leo, R.A. (2006). Strategies for preventing false confessions and their
consequences. In M.R. Kebbell & G.M. Davies (Eds.), Practical psychology for forensic
investigations and prosecutions (pp. 121-149). West Sussex, England: John Wiley &
Sons, Ltd.

109
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Drizin, S.A., & Leo, R.A. (2004). The problem of false confessions in the post-DNA world.
North Carolina Law Review, 82, 891-1007.
Findley, K.A. (2011). Defining innocence. Albany Law Review, 74, 1157-1208.
Findley, K.A., & Scott, M. (2006). The multiple dimensions of tunnel vision in criminal cases.
Wisconsin Law Review, 2006, 291-397.
Frank, J., & Frank, B. (1957). Not guilty. Garden City, NY: Doubleday & Company, Inc.
Free, M.D., & Ruesink, M. (2012). Race and justice: Wrongful convictions of African American
men. Boulder, CO: Lynne Rienner Publishers.
Long, J.S., & Freese, J. (2006). Regression models for categorical dependent variables using
Stata, 2nd ed. College Station: Stata Press.
Frye v. United States, 293 F. 1013 (D.C. Cir. 1923).
Gardner, E.S. (1952). The court of last resort. New York, NY: William Sloane Associates.
Garrett, B.L. (2008). Judging innocence. Columbia Law Review, 108, 55-141.
Garrett, B.L. (2011). Convicting the innocent: Where criminal prosecutions go wrong.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Garrett, B.L., & Neufeld, P.J. (2009). Invalid forensic science testimony and wrongful
convictions. Virginia Law Review, 95(1), 1-97.
Gould, J. (2007). The Innocence Commission: Preventing wrongful convictions and restoring the
criminal justice system. New York, NY: New York University Press
Gould, J., & Leo, R.A. (2010). One hundred years later: Wrongful convictions after a century of
research. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 100(3), 825-868.

110
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Gould, J., & Greenman, L. (2010). Update on the cost and quality of defense representation in
federal death penalty cases. Committee on Defender Services of the Judicial Conference
of the United States.
Grisham, J. (2006). The innocent man: Murder and injustice in a small town. New York, NY:
Doubleday.
Gross, S.R. (1996). The risks of death: Why erroneous convictions are common in capital cases.
Buffalo Law Review, 44, 469-500.
Gross, S.R. (1998). Lost lives: Miscarriages of justice in capital cases. Law and Contemporary
Problems, 61(Aut.), 125-152.
Gross, S.R., & O’Brien, B. (2008). Frequency and predictors of false convictions: Why we know
so little, and new data on capital cases. The Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 5, 927962.
Gross, S.R., & Shaffer, M. (2012). Exonerations in the United States, 1989-2012. National
Registry of Exonerations. Retrieved from http://www.law.umich.edu/special/
exoneration/Documents/exonerations_us_1989_2012_full_report.pdf
Gross, S.R., Jacoby, K., Matheson, D.J., Montgomery, N., & Patil, S. (2005). Exonerations in the
United States 1989 through 2003. Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 95, 523560.
Gudjonsson, G.H. (2003). The psychology of interrogations and confessions: A handbook. West
Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Haber, L., & Haber, R.N. (2008). Scientific validation of fingerprint evidence under Daubert.
Law, Probability, and Risk, 7, 87-106.

111
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Harmon, T.R. (2001). Predictors of miscarriages of justice in capital cases. Justice Quarterly,
18(4), 949-968.
Harmon, T.R., & Lofquist, W.S. (2005). Too late for luck: A comparison of post-Furman
exonerations and executions of the innocent. Crime and Delinquency, 51, 498-520.
Harris, D.A. (1999). The stories, the statistics, and the law: Why “Driving While Black” matters.
Minnesota Law Review, 84, 265-326.
Hartwig, M., Granhag, P.A., Strömwall, L.A., & Vrij, A. (2004). Police officers’ lie detection
accuracy: Interrogating freely versus observing video. Police Quarterly, 7(4), 429-456.
Holmes, W.M. (2002). Wrongful convictions in capital cases and the legacy of lynching. Paper
presented at the American Society of Criminology Annual Meetings, Chicago, IL.
Horton, N. J., & Kleinman, K.P. (2007). Much ado about nothing: A comparison of missing
data methods and software to fit incomplete data regression models. American
Statistician, 61, 79–90.
House v. Bell, 547 U.S. 518 (2006).
Huff, R., Rattner, A., & Sagarin E. (1996). Convicted but innocent: Wrongful conviction and
public policy. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.
Innocence Network. (n.d.) Retrieved December 6, 2012 from http://www.innocencenetwork.org/
Innocence Project. (n.d.) Retrieved December 6, 2012 from http://www.innocenceproject.org/
Innocence Protection Act of 2004, 18 U.S.C. § 3600 (2006).
Kansas v. Marsh, 548 U.S. 163 (2006).
Kassin, S.M., & Fong, C.T. (1999). “I’m Innocent!” Effects of training on judgments of truth and
deception in the interrogation room. Law and Human Behavior, 23(5), 499-516.

112
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Kassin, S.M. (2012). Why confessions trump innocence. American Psychologist (advance online
publication Apr. 30, 2012).
Kassin, S.M., Drizin, S.A., Grisso, T., Gudjonsson, G.H., Leo, R.A., & Redlich, A.D. (2010).
Police-induced confessions: Risk factors and recommendations. Law and Human
Behavior, 34, 3-38.
King, G., Honaker, J., Joseph, A., & Scheve, K. (2001). Analyzing incomplete political science
data: An alternative algorithm for multiple imputation. American Political Science
Review, 95, 49–69.
King, G., Tomz, M., & Wittenberg, J. (2000). Making the most of statistical analyses: Improving
interpretation and presentation. American Journal of Political Science, 44, 347–361.
Kutateladze, B. (2009). Is America Really So Punitive? Exploring a Continuum of U.S. State
Criminal Justice Policies. El Paso, TX: LFB Scholarly Publishing.
Lain, C.B. (2007). Deciding death. Duke Law Journal, 57, 1-83.
Leo, R.A. (2005). Re-thinking the study of miscarriages of justice: Developing a criminology of
wrongful convictions. Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 21, 201-223.
Leo, R.A. (2008). Police interrogation and American justice. Cambridge, MA: Harvard
University Press.
Leo, R.A. (2009). False confessions: causes, consequences, and implications. The Journal of the
American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law, 37, 332-343.
Leo, R.A., & Drizin, S.A. (2010). The three errors: Pathways to false confession and wrongful
conviction. In G.D. Lassiter, & C.A. Meissner (Eds.), Police interrogations and false
confessions (pp. 9-30). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

113
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Leo, R.A., & Drizin, S.A., Neufeld, P.J., Hall, B.R., & Vatner, A. (2006). Bringing reliability
back in: False confessions and legal safeguards in the Twenty-First Century. Wisconsin
Law Review, 2006, 479-538.
Leo, R.A., & Gould, J.B. (2009). Studying wrongful convictions: Learning from social science.
Ohio State Journal of Criminal Law, 7, 7-30.
Leo, R.A., & Liu, B. (2009). What do potential jurors know about police interrogation
techniques and false confessions? Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 27(3), 381-399.
Doi:10.1002/bsl.872
Leo, R.A., & Ofshe, R.J. (1998). The consequences of false confessions: Deprivations of liberty
and miscarriages of justice in the age of psychological interrogation. The Journal of
Criminal Law and Criminology, 88, 429-496.
Liebman, J.S., Fagan, J., Gelman, A., West, V., Davies, G., & Kiss, A. (2002). A broken system,
part II: Why there is so much error in capital cases, and what can be done about it.
Retrieved from http://www2.law.columbia.edu/brokensystem2/appendix.html.
Liebman, J.S., Fagan, J., West, V., & Lloyd, J. (2000). Capital attrition: Error rates in capital
cases, 1973-1995. Texas Law Review, 78, 1839-1865.
Little, J. R., & Rubin, D.B. (1987). Statistical Analysis with Missing Data. New York: J. Wiley
& Sons.
Little, J. R., and Rubin, D. (1989). The analysis of social science data with missing values.
Sociological Methods and Research, 18, 292–326.
Lofquist, W. (2001). Whodunit? An examination of the production of wrongful convictions. In S.
Westervelt & J. Humphrey (Eds.), Wrongly convicted: Perspectives on failed justice
(pp.174-198). Newark, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

114
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Loftus, E.F., & Ketcham, K. (1991). Witness for the defense: The accused, the eyewitness, and
the expert who puts memory on trial. New York, NY: St. Martin's Press.
Markman, S.J., & Cassell, P.G. (1988). Protecting the innocent: A response to the Bedau-Radelet
study. Stanford Law Review, 41, 121-160.
Martin, D. (2002). Lessons about justice from the “laboratory” of wrongful convictions: Tunnel
vision, the construction of guilt and informer evidence. University of Missouri-Kansas
City School of Law Review, 70, 847-864.
Medwed, D.S. (2004). The zeal deal: Prosecutorial resistance to post-conviction claims of
innocence. Boston University Law Review, 84, 125-183.
Medwed, D.S. (2008). Innocentrism. University of Illinois Law Review, 2008, 1549-1560.
Medwed, D.S. (2012). Prosecution complex: America’s race to convict and its impact on the
innocent. New York, NY: New York University Press.
Meissner, C.A., & Brigham, J.C. (2001). Thirty years of investigating own-race bias in memory
for faces: A meta-analysis. Psychology, Public Policy, & Law, 7, 3-35.
Merola, M. (1982). Federal, state, and local governments: Partners in the fight against violent
crime. The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 73, 965-984.
Mitchell, T.L., Haw, R.M, Pfeifer, J.E., & Meissner, C.A. (2005). Racial bias in mock juror
decision-making: A meta-analytic review of defendant treatment. Law and Human
Behavior, 29(6), 621-637.
Mnookin, J. (2008). The validity of latent fingerprint identification: Confessions of a
fingerprinting moderate. Law, Probability, and Risk, 7, 127-141.
Moenssens, A.A. (1993). Novel scientific evidence in criminal cases: Some words of caution.
Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology, 84, 1-21.

115
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Natapoff, A. (2006). Beyond unreliable: How snitches contribute to wrongful convictions.
Golden Gate University Law Review, 37, 107-129.
National Institute of Justice. (1999). Eyewitness evidence: A guide for law enforcement.
Washington, D.C.: U.S. Dept. of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, National Institute of
Justice.
National Research Council. (2009). Strengthening forensic science in the United States: A path
forward. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press.
National Transportation Safety Board. (1973). Accident report (File No. 1-0016) concerning
Eastern Airlines flight 401 Lockheed L-1011-1 N310EA.Washington, DC: National
Transportation Safety Board.
Ofshe, R.J., & Leo, R.A. (1997). The social psychology of police interrogation: The theory and
classification of true and false confessions. Studies in Law, Politics, & Society, 16, 189251.
Pratt, T. C., Gaffney, M. J., Lovrich, N. P., & Johnson, C. L. (2006). This isn’t CSI: Estimating
the backlog of forensic DNA cases and the barriers associated with case processing.
Criminal Justice Policy Review, 17, 32-47.
Radin, E.D. (1964). The Innocents. New York, NY: William Morrow and Company.
Raeder, M. (2003). What does innocence have to do with it?: A commentary on wrongful
convictions and rationality. Michigan State Law Review, 2003, 1315-1335.
Redlich, A.D. (2004). Mental illness, police interrogations, and the potential for false confession.
Law and Psychiatry, 55, 19-21. Doi:10.1176/appi.ps.55.1.19
Roman, J., Walsh, K., Lachman, P., & Jahner, J. (2012). Post-Conviction DNA testing and
wrongful conviction. Urban Institute Report. Washington, DC.

116
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1978). Interpersonal expectancy effects: The first 345 studies.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 377-386.
Royston, P. (2004). Multiple imputation of missing values. The Stata Journal, 4(3), 227-241.
Rubin, D.B. (1987). Multiple imputation for survey nonresponse. New York: Wiley.
Rubin, D. (1977). Formalizing subjective notions about the effect of nonrespondents in sample
surveys. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 72, 538–43.
Rutter, M. (2007). Proceeding from observed correlation to causal inference: The use of natural
experiments. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 377-395. Doi:10.1111/j.17456916.2007.00050.x
Salas, C. (2004). The case for excluding the criminal confessions of the mentally ill. Yale
Journal of Law and the Humanities, 16, 243-276.
Schafer, J.L. (1997). Analysis of incomplete multivariate data. Chapman & Hall, London.
Scheck, B., Neufeld, P., & Dwyer, J. (2000). Actual innocence: Five days to execution and other
dispatches from the wrongly convicted. New York, NY: Doubleday.
Schroeder, D.A., & White, M.D. (2009). Exploring the use of DNA evidence in homicide
investigations: Implications for detective work and case clearance. Police Quarterly, 12,
319-342.
Simon, D. (2012). In doubt: The psychology of the criminal justice process. Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press.
Staw, B.M. (1981). The escalation of commitment to a course of action. Academy of
Management Review, 6, 577-587.

117
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Steblay, N., Dysart, J., Fulero, S., & Lindsay, R.C.L. (2001). Eyewitness accuracy rates in
sequential and simultaneous lineup presentations: A meta-analytic comparison. Law and
Human Behavior, 25(5), 459-473. Doi:10.1023/A:1012888715007
Sullivan, T.P. (2005). Electronic recording of custodial interrogations: Everybody wins. Journal
of Criminal Law and Criminology, 95, 1127-1144.
Thomas, G.C. (2008). The Supreme Court on trial. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan
Press.
Tonry, M.H. (1995) Malign neglect: Race, crime, and punishment in the United States. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Trott, S.S. (1996). Words of warning for prosecutors using criminals as witnesses. Hastings Law
Journal, 47, 1381-1432.
Van Buuren, S., Boshuizen, H.C., & Knook. D.L. (1999). Multiple imputation of missing blood
pressure covariates in survival analysis. Statistics in Medicine, 18, 681-694.
Ward, M.D., Greenhill, B.D., & Bakke, K.B. (2010). The perils of policy by p-value: predicting
civil conflicts.
Journal of Peace Research, 47(4), 363-375.
Warden, R. (2003). The role of false confessions in Illinois wrongful murder convictions since
1970. Center on Wrongful Convictions Research Report. Retrieved from
http://www.law.northwestern.edu/wrongfulconvictions/issues/causesandremedies/falseco
nfessions/FalseConfessionsStudy.html
Wells, G.L., & Bradfield, A.L. (1998). Good, you identified the suspect: Feedback to eyewitness
distorts their reports of the witnessing experience. Journal of Applied Psychology, 83(3),
360-376.

118
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Wells, G.L., & Murray, D.M. (1983). What can Psychology say about the Neil v. Biggers criteria
for judging eyewitness accuracy? Journal of Applied Psychology, 68(3), 347-362.
Wells, G.L. (2008). Theory, logic, and data: Paths to a more coherent eyewitness science.
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22(6), 853-859.
Wells, G.L., & Olson, E. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review Psychology, 54, 277295.
Wells, G.L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R.S., Fulero, S.M., & Brimacombe, C.A.E. (1998).
Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photospreads.
Law and Human Behavior, 22(6), 603.
White, W.S. (2003). Miranda’s waning protections: Police interrogation practices after
Dickerson. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.
Yant, M. (1991). Presumed guilty: When innocent people are wrongly convicted. Buffalo, NY:
Prometheus Books.
Zalman, M. (2011). An integrated justice model of wrongful convictions. Albany Law Review,
74, 1465-1524.
Zalman, M. (2012). Qualitatively estimating the incidence of wrongful convictions. Criminal
Law Bulletin, 48(2), 221-279.
Zimmerman, C. (2001). From the jailhouse to the courthouse: The role of informants in wrongful
convictions. In S.D. Westervelt & J.A. Humphrey (Eds.), Wrongly convicted:
Perspectives on failed justice (pp. 17-85). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

119
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

VII. TABLES AND FIGURES

Figure 1. Distribution of Cases by County Population
u.s. Counties Population (Continental ):

.•

•

•

,

•

.,
l egend
Near Mines

,.,
...

. ,, 1

••

•

. • .'
.• • • •'. .,' \9,.•
•
• r
•
•
..' ..
• •• •

.

•

• ••

"l- •

••

•

•
•

... •

.

,,

•

Wrongful Conviction and Near Miss Occurence Frequency

),-,

• 'I

,

,

,

"

(. ~

• \ r#.

••

,

J . .. . .

,

••

,,~

4 ·8

•

.&. 7-9
' " 100,g ,. .ltr

...

Wrongful Convic tions
•

1·3

e H

.

100';'''10'

U.S. Counties Population 2007
55·220904
22090:i·7~115

. 7~116.2100707
•
_

2100703 ~OH27
SoI07428· 10110975

120
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 1. Erroneous Convictions by State

State 

Frequency 

Percent 

AZ 

2 

.8 

CA 

13 

5.0 

CT 

3 

1.2 

DC 

2 

.8 

FL 

9 

3.5 

GA 

5 

1.9 

ID 

1 

.4 

IL 

35 

13.5 

IN 

5 

1.9 

KS 

2 

.8 

KY 

1 

.4 

LA 

8 

3.1 

MA 

8 

3.1 

MD 

2 

.8 

MI 

4 

1.5 

MN 

1 

.4 

MO 

8 

3.1 

MS 

2 

.8 

MT 

2 

.8 

NC 

9 

3.5 

NE 

6 

2.3 

121
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

NJ 

5 

1.9 

NY 

29 

11.2 

OH 

10 

3.8 

OK 

6 

2.3 

OR 

4 

1.5 

PA 

8 

3.1 

RI 

2 

.8 

SC 

1 

.4 

TN 

3 

1.2 

TX 

40 

15.4 

VA 

15 

5.8 

WA 

1 

.4 

WI 

5 

1.9 

WV 

3 

1.2 

Total 

260 

100.0 

 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 

122
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 2. Near Misses by State
 
State 

Frequency 

Percent 

AK 

2 

1.0 

AL 

2 

1.0 

AZ 

6 

3.0 

CA 

24 

12.0 

CO 

1 

.5 

CT 

1 

.5 

DC 

4 

2.0 

FL 

18 

9.0 

GA 

2 

1.0 

IL 

20 

10.0 

IN 

3 

1.5 

KY 

2 

1.0 

MA 

2 

1.0 

MD 

5 

2.5 

ME 

1 

.5 

MI 

6 

3.0 

MN 

2 

1.0 

MO 

12 

6.0 

MS 

1 

.5 

NC 

5 

2.5 

123
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

ND 

1 

.5 

NE 

2 

1.0 

NH 

1 

.5 

NJ 

2 

1.0 

NM 

1 

.5 

NV 

1 

.5 

NY 

17 

8.5 

OH 

4 

2.0 

OK 

1 

.5 

OR 

2 

1.0 

PA 

18 

9.0 

RI 

2 

1.0 

SC 

1 

.5 

TN 

2 

1.0 

TX 

8 

4.0 

UT 

4 

2.0 

VA 

3 

1.5 

WA 

10 

5.0 

WI 

1 

.5 

Total 

200 

100.0 

124
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 3. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Location Effects
Location Effect Variables

Erroneous Convictions Total
% (N)
35% (92)

Near Misses Total
%(N)
21% (42)

.07 (201)

0.4 (160)

47% (122)

59% (117)

Crime rate 5 years prior

609 (260)

636 (190)

Crime rate the year of the crime

643 (260)

643 (178)

Crime consistency**

6.5 (260)

-0.8 (178)

Case from former Confederate state***
Death penalty culture**
Political ideology of state (Median
Democratic presidential vote 1980 to 2008)*

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

Note: The following variables are not represented as percentages: death penalty culture, crime rate 5 years prior, crime rate the
year of the crime; and crime consistency.

125
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 4. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Nature of the Defendant
Nature of the Defendant Variables

Erroneous Convictions Total
% (N)
57 (257)

Near Misses Total
% (N)
37 (189)

High school graduate**

45 (128)

65 (105)

Previous criminal conviction***

67 (212)

42 (166)

17 (132)

25 (65)

Female defendant*

2 (260)

6 (200)

Mean age of defendant***

25 (253)

29 (193)

Gang affiliation

4 (253)

7 (199)

Mental disorder

15 (254)

11 (199)

None:

23 (222)

46 (174)

Weak:

54 (222)

32 (174)

Probative:

19 (222)

18 (174)

Highly probative:

5 (222)

4 (174)

Race of defendant/% African American***

If so, for similar crime

Strength of suspect characteristics***

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

Note: The following variables are not represented as percentages: mean age of defendant.  

126
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 5. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Nature of the Crime
 
Nature of the Crime Variables

Erroneous Convictions Total
% (N)
65 (198)

Near Misses Total
% (N)
62 (155)

Race of victim 1 differs from
defendant 1*

43 (198)

31 (151)

Victim 1 and defendant 1 were
strangers**

65 (251)

48 (198)

No victim was a stranger***

62 (260)

46 (200)

Female victim 1***

85 (260)

57 (199)

At least one female victim***

86 (260)

66 (199)

At least one white female victim***

48 (260)

31 (200)

Murder only

28 (260)

56 (200)

Rape only

56 (260)

26 (200)

Murder and rape

14 (260)

5 (200)

Lesser offense

5 (260)

11 (200)

Mean number of charges

4 (243)

5 (200)

Serial crime

25 (259)

25 (199)

Criminal justice action demanded*

8 (260)

15 (200)

Mean number of alleged coperpetrators**

0.7 (260)

1.5 (200)

Mean number of codefendants**

0.2 (260)

0.7 (200)

Mean number of female victims*

1.0 (260)

0.9 (200)

Mean number of white female
victims*

0.5 (260)

0.4 (200)

Race of victim 1 (% Caucasian)

Criminal charges***

127
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Strength of victim characteristics
None:

37 (260)

53 (200)

Weak:

4 (260)

2 (200)

Probative:

28 (260)

23 (200)

Highly probative:

32 (260)

22 (200)

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

Note: The following variables are not represented as percentages: mean number of charges, mean number of
alleged co-perpetrators, mean number of codefendants, mean number of female victims, and mean number of white
female victims.  

128
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 6. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Quality of Work by Criminal Justice Officials
 
Quality of Work Variables

Erroneous Convictions Total
% (N)
23 (246)

Near Misses Total
% (N)
32 (199)

Local police investigated*

81 (246)

72 (199)

State bureau investigated

1 (249)

4 (199)

State police investigated*

3 (248)

7 (199)

Federal law enforcement investigated*

.04 (257)

3 (200)

Exculpatory evidence withheld*

14 (259)

6 (200)

[Alleged: 7]

[Alleged: 7]

Police withheld exculpatory evidence

11 (259)

9 (199)

Prosecution withheld exculpatory

11 (260)

4 (200)

evidence**

1.3 (226)

1.2 (178)

Time b/w crime and arrest (days-log)

1.8 (116)

1.6 (73)

5 (26)

0.5 (200)

[Alleged: 5]

[Alleged: 3]

0.4 (260)

0 (200)

[Alleged: 0.8]

[Alleged: 0]

0.4 (260)

9 (199)

16 (259)

11 (198)

[Alleged: 14]

[Alleged: 15]

8 (259)

12 (199)

[Alleged: 9]

[Alleged: 15]

9 (260)

6 (199)

[Alleged: 7]

[Alleged: 6]

4 (260)

3 (200)

[Alleged: 5]

[Alleged: 4]

County sheriff investigated*

Time b/w arrest and indictment (days-log)**
Judicial error**

Judicial misconduct

Medical error***
Police error

Police misconduct

Prosecutor error

Prosecutor misconduct

129
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Other official error

Other official misconduct

Criminal justice official lying**

Forensic fraud

Abuse to elicit confession

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

0.8 (260)

0.5 (200)

[Alleged: 0]

[Alleged: 0.5]

0.4 (260)

0 (200)

[Alleged: 0.8]

[Alleged: 0]

5 (259)

1 (199)

[Alleged: 4]

[Alleged: 1]

6 (203)

4 (138)

[Alleged: 6]

[Alleged: 4]

21 (57)

18 (56)

***p<0.001

Note: The following variables are not represented as percentages: time between crime and arrest and time between
arrest and indictment.

130
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 7. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Nature of the Facts Available to the State
Nature of the Facts Variables

Erroneous Convictions Total
% (N)
14 (259)

Near Misses Total
% (N)
22 (200)

Any incriminating statement

22 (259)

29 (200)

False evidence by jailhouse snitch

11 (260)

7 (200)

Non-eyewitness gave evidence*

31 (260)

21 (198)

Eyewitness identification

82 (260)

75 (200)

Eyewitness misidentification***
(unintentional, only cases with ID)

83 (215)

57 (151)

Additional misidentification***

20 (260)

9 (199)

Intentional misidentification or lie by
victim***

2 (260)

13 (200)

Intentional misidentification by non-victim
eyewitness***

12 (260)

24 (200)

Intentional misidentification***

14 (260)

36 (200)

Forensic evidence errors***
(only cases with forensics)

43 (203)

22 (133)

Forensic evidence errors (all cases)***

34 (260)

15 (200)

Presented w/ no errors:

18 (258)

9 (196)

Errors:

1 (258)

2 (196)

Presented w/ no errors:

0.4 (259)

13 (195)

Errors:

0.8 (259)

0 (195)

Presented w/ no errors:

20 (258)

3 (196)

Errors:

6 (258)

1 (196)

False confession*

If so, was evidence false?

Fingerprint*

Ballistics***

Microscopic hair***

131
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Serology***
Presented w/ no errors:

29 (259)

4 (196)

Errors in testing:

3 (259)

0.5 (196)

Errors in testimony:

16 (259)

0.5 (196)

Errors in testing and testimony

2 (259)

0 (196)

Presented w/ no errors:

3 (259)

25 (196)

Errors:

1 (259)

2 (196)

0.4 (259)

0 (196)

2 (259)

2 (196)

Victim’s property near defendant

3 (260)

4 (200)

Anonymous tip

3 (260)

5 (200)

Surveillance/wiretap evidence***

1 (260)

8 (200)

Misleading circumstantial evidence**

35 (260

48 (200)

Any victim recanted crime***

0 (260)

9 (200)

0.4 (260)

13 (200)

Any non-victim eyewitness recanted***

5 (260)

22 (200)

Any witness recanted crime or ID***

10 (260)

34 (200)

Victim 1 provided description of perp

94 (123)

97 (66)

Victim 1 took multiple tries to make ID

13 (145)

7 (68)

Victim 1 certain of ID

82 (137)

82 (67)

Victim 1 misidentified the defendant*
(only cases where victim was alive)

92 (158)

83 (82)

Discrepancy b/w victim 1’s description and
defendant***

56 (101)

21 (63)

Discrepancy b/w any victim’s description
and defendant***

22 (206)

8 (200)

DNA***

Bite-mark
Presented w/ no errors:
Errors:

Any victim recanted identification***

132
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Victim 1 provided unique details of perp*

35 (106)

54 (63)

Victim 1 made cross-racial ID*

58 (100)

35 (43)

Direct:

22 (125)

56 (63)

Show-up:

8 (125)

5 (63)

Photo array:

58 (125)

37 (63)

Lineup:

10 (125)

3 (63)

Single photo:

2 (125)

0 (63)

36 (258)

49 (183)

Eyewitness 1 took multiple tries to ID
defendant*

8 (92)

1 (88)

Eyewitness 1 provided unique details of perp

66 (62)

71 (76)

Discrepancy b/w Eyewitness 1’s description
and the defendant

19 (62)

9 (75)

Eyewitness 1 made cross-racial ID

21 (57)

19 (54)

Direct:

48 (86)

65 (89)

Show-up:

2 (86)

6 (89)

Photo array:

35 (86)

21 (89)

Lineup:

11 (86)

7 (89)

Single photo:

5 (86)

1 (89)

Eyewitness 1 provided description of perp

89 (66)

96 (82)

Eyewitness 1 certain of ID

82 (87)

89 (89)

1.3 (260)

1.5 (200)

None:

43 (260)

32 (200)

Weak:

7 (260)

2 (200)

Victim 1 identification method***

Eyewitness 1 misidentified defendant**

Eyewitness 1 identification method

Total number of eyewitnesses
Strength of witness characteristics**

133
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Probative:

25 (260)

27 (200)

Highly probative:

26 (260)

39 (200)

None:

10 (259)

17 (200)

Weak:

17 (259)

16 (200)

Probative:

54 (259)

37 (200)

Highly probative:

19 (259)

31 (200)

None:

73 (260)

61 (200)

Weak:

14 (260)

20 (200)

Probative:

12 (260)

13 (200)

Highly probative:

1 (260)

8 (200)

None:

18 (260)

24 (200)

Weak:

52 (260)

54 (200)

Probative:

27 (260)

17 (200)

Highly probative:

4 (260)

6 (200)

Weak:

35 (260)

24 (200)

Probative:

53 (260)

46 (200)

Highly probative:

12 (260)

31 (200)

Strength of identification information**

Strength of suspect statements

Strength of physical evidence

Strength of prosecution’s case***

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

Note: The following variables are not represented as percentages: total number of eyewitnesses.

134
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 8. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Quality of Defense
 
Quality of Defense Variables

Erroneous Convictions Total
% (N)
73 (153)

Near Misses Total
% (N)
61 (133)

Defense presented alternative suspect***

10 (192)

22 (157)

Defense presented DNA evidence*

1 (193)

4 (159)

Defense presented defendant’s friend as a
witness

36 (180)

26 (152)

Defense presented disinterested person as a
witness

28 (180)

28 (153)

Defense presented evidence of
misconduct***

7 (193)

20 (163)

Defense presented expert witness

17 (193)

22 (158)

Defense presented exculpatory evidence

66 (196)

73 (161)

Defense presented family member as a
witness***

52 (181)

25 (153)

Defense presented physical evidence
corroborating alibi***

5 (194)

24 (153)

Defense presented evidence of mental illness
or disability

5 (193)

3 (159)

4 (259)
[Alleged: 14]

0 (200)

Attorney was a public defender*

Substantiated allegations of incompetent
defense counsel***
* p<0.05
** p<0.01
***p<0.001

135
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 9. Erroneous Convictions by Year of Crime

Date of Crime† 

Frequency 

Percent 

1973 

1 

.4 

1977 

1 

.4 

1978 

1 

.4 

1979 

5 

1.9 

1980 

7 

2.7 

1981 

20 

7.7 

1982 

23 

8.8 

1983 

20 

7.7 

1984 

18 

6.9 

1985 

25 

9.6 

1986 

26 

10.0 

1987 

16 

6.2 

1988 

8 

3.1 

1989 

16 

6.2 

1990 

17 

6.5 

1991 

12 

4.6 

1992 

7 

2.7 

1993 

5 

1.9 

1994 

4 

1.5 

1995 

3 

1.2 

1996 

8 

3.1 

136
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1997 

7 

2.7 

1998 

3 

1.2 

1999 

1 

.4 

2001 

1 

.4 

2002 

1 

.4 

2005 

3 

1.2 

2006 

1 

.4 

Total 

260 

100.0 

†If the case involved more than one crime, the year of the most recent crime was used.

137
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 10. Near Misses by Year of Crime

Date of Crime† 

Frequency 

Percent 

1973 

1 

.5 

1979 

1 

.5 

1980 

3 

1.5 

1981 

1 

.5 

1982 

1 

.5 

1983 

5 

2.5 

1984 

8 

4.0 

1985 

7 

3.5 

1986 

5 

2.5 

1987 

1 

.5 

1988 

2 

1.0 

1989 

2 

1.0 

1990 

7 

3.5 

1991 

16 

8.0 

1992 

5 

2.5 

1993 

3 

1.5 

1994 

4 

2.0 

1995 

2 

1.0 

1996 

4 

2.0 

1997 

8 

4.0 

1998 

13 

6.5 

138
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1999 

4 

2.0 

2000 

14 

7.0 

2001 

4 

2.0 

2002 

8 

4.0 

2003 

5 

2.5 

2004 

4 

2.0 

2005 

9 

4.5 

2006 

9 

4.5 

2007 

4 

2.0 

2008 

10 

5.0 

2009 

11 

5.5 

2010 

13 

6.5 

2011 

6 

3.0 

Total 

200 

100.0 

†If the case involved more than one crime, the year of the most recent crime was used.

139
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 11. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Defendant, Significant Variables Controlled for
Murder and Sexual Assault
Erroneous
Convictions
Murder Cases
%(N)

Near Misses
Murder Cases
% (N)

Race of defendant (%
African American)

45 (109)

High school graduate

Near Misses
Sexual Assault Cases
%(N)

39 (124)

Erroneous
Convictions
Sexual Assault
Cases
%(N)
62 (182)***

42 (60)

54 (76)

44 (97)***

82 (28)***

Previous criminal
conviction

65 (81)**

46 (109)**

66 (157)***

38 (53)***

Female defendant

5 (109)

5 (131)

1 (185)*

5 (67)*

25 (106)**

28 (128)**

25 (65)**

30 (181)**

26 (85)

43 (118)

24 (161)**

50 (54)**

Weak:

57 (85)

32 (118)

53 (161)

33 (54)

Probative:

14 (85)

21 (118)

19 (161)

17 (54)

Highly probative:

4 (85)

3 (118)

4 (161)

0 (54)

Nature of the Defendant
Variables

Mean age of defendant

Strength of suspect
characteristics
None:

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

31 (62)***

† no control applied

Note: The following variables are not represented as percentages: mean age of defendant.

140
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 12. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Defendant, Significant Variables Controlled for
Pre- and Post-DNA Resolution
Erroneous
Convictions
Pre-DNA Cases
%(N)
65 (147)***

Near Misses
Pre-DNA Cases
% (N)

Near Misses
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)

39 (26)***

Erroneous
Convictions
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)
47 (109)

48 (75)

62 (13)

40 (53)*

65 (92)*

65 (130)***

37 (19)***

68 (82)*

43 (147)*

None:

25 (134)

44 (23)

22 (88)**

46 (151)**

Weak:

52 (134)

23 (23)

57 (88)

34 (151)

Probative:

19 (134)

30 (23)

18 (88)

16 (151)

Highly probative:

5 (134)

4 (23)

3 (88)

4 (151)

Nature of the Defendant
Variables
Race of defendant (%
African American)
High school graduate
Previous criminal conviction

37 (163)

Strength of suspect
characteristics

Female defendant†
Mean age of defendant†
* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

† no control applied

141
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 13. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Crime, Significant Variables Controlled for
Murder and Sexual Assault
Erroneous
Convictions
Murder Cases
%(N)

Near Misses
Murder
Cases
% (N)

52 (52)

Near Misses
Sexual Assault
Cases
%(N)

47 (61)

Erroneous
Convictions
Sexual Assault
Cases
%(N)
69 (182)***

69 (109)***

44 (131)***

98 (185)*

93 (67)*

At least one white female victim

44 (109)*

31 (131)*

58 (185)*

40 (67)*

Criminal justice action demanded

14 (15)

15 (19)

7 (185)***

21 (67)***

1.3 (109)

1.1 (131)

0.4 (185)*

0.9 (67)*

Nature of the Crime Variables

Victim 1 and defendant 1 were
strangers
Female victim 1

Mean number of alleged coperpetrators

38 (66)***

Mean number of codefendants†
Mean number of female victims†
Mean number of white female
victims†
Additive charges include murder†
At least one female victim†
No victim was a stranger†
Race of victim 1 differs from
defendant 1†
* p<0.05 ** p<0.01
***p<0.001

† no control applied

Note: The following variables are not represented as percentages: mean number of alleged co-perpetrators. Other
categories with “mean number” also do not depict percentages.

142
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 14. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Crime, Significant Variables Controlled for
Pre- and Post-DNA Resolution
Erroneous
Convictions
Pre-DNA Cases
%(N)
70 (12)

Near Misses
Pre-DNA
Cases
% (N)
64 (3)

Erroneous
Convictions
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)
57 (60)

Near Misses
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)

Additive charges include murder

20 (29)*

50 (14)*

34 (38)*

57 (98)*

Mean number of alleged coperpetrators

0.5 (148)

0.7 (28)

1.7 (112)***

0.9 (172)***

Nature of the Crime Variables

Victim 1 and defendant 1 were
strangers

45 (77)

Mean number of codefendants†
Criminal justice action demanded†
Mean number of female victims†
Mean number of white female
victims†
Race of victim 1 differs from
defendant 1†
No victim was a stranger†
Female victim 1†
At least one female victim†
At least one white female victim†
* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

†no control applied

Note: The following variables are not represented as percentages: mean number of alleged co-perpetrators. Other
categories with “mean number” also do not depict percentages.

143
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 15. Bivariate Results: Quality of Work by Criminal Justice Officials, Significant
Variables Controlled for Murder and Sexual Assault

Quality of Work Variables

Exculpatory evidence withheld

Medical error
Prosecution withheld exculpatory
evidence

Erroneous
Convictions
Murder Cases
% (N)

Near Misses
Murder
Cases
% (N)

19 (108)***
[Alleged: 9]

Near Misses
Sexual Assault
Cases
% (N)

5 (131)***
[Alleged: 8]

Erroneous
Convictions
Sexual Assault
Cases
% (N)
10 (184)
[Alleged: 6]

1 (109)**

9 (130)**

0 (185)***

9 (67)***

19 (108)***

2 (130)***

7 (184)

6 (67)

9 (67)
[Alleged: 6]

Criminal justice official lying†
County sheriff investigated†
Local police investigated†
State police investigated†
Federal law enforcement
investigated†
Time b/w arrest and indictment
(days-log)†
Judicial error†
* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

† no control applied

144
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 16. Bivariate Results: Quality of Work by Criminal Justice Officials, Significant
Variables Controlled for Pre- and Post-DNA Resolution
Quality of Work Variables

Exculpatory evidence
withheld
Prosecution withheld
exculpatory evidence

Erroneous
Convictions
Pre-DNA Cases
%(N)
12 (147)
[Alleged: 8]

Near Misses
Pre-DNA Cases
% (N)

9 (147)

Near Misses
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)

4 (28)
[Alleged: 4]

Erroneous
Convictions
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)
15 (112)
[Alleged: 5]

0 (28)

13 (112)**

4 (171)**

6 (171)
[Alleged: 7]

Criminal justice official
lying†
Medical error†
County sheriff investigated†
Local police investigated†
State police investigated†
Federal law enforcement
investigated†
Time b/w arrest and
indictment (days-log)†
Judicial error†
* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

† no control applied

145
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 17. Bivariate Results: Quality of Work by Criminal Justice Officials, Significant
Variables Controlled for Illinois
Erroneous
Convictions
Illinois
Excluded
% (N)
24 (211)*

Near Misses
Illinois
Excluded
% (N)

Local police investigated

80 (211)*

71 (180)*

State police investigated

3 (213)

7 (180)

Federal law enforcement
investigated

0.5 (222)*

3 (180)*

Quality of Work
Variables

County sheriff
investigated

33 (180)*

Criminal justice official
lying†
Time b/w arrest and
indictment (days-log)†
Judicial error†
Exculpatory evidence
withheld†
Prosecution withheld
exculpatory evidence†
Medical error†
* p<0.05

** p<0.01

146
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 18. Bivariate Results: Location Effects, Significant Variables Controlled for Illinois
Location Effect Variables
Case from former Confederate state***

Erroneous Convictions Illinois
Excluded
(N)
41% (225)

Near Misses Illinois
Excluded
(N)
23% (180)

Death penalty culture***

.081 (166)

Political ideology of state (Median
Democratic presidential vote 1980 to
2008)**
Crime rate 5 years prior

39% (225)

54% (180)

581 (225)

620 (170)

Crime rate the year of the crime

614 (225)

628 (159)

Crime consistency***

6.57 (225)

-0.71 (159)

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

.039 (140)

***p<0.001

147
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 19. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Facts Available to the State, Significant
Variables Controlled for Murder and Sexual Assault
Nature of the Facts
Variables

False confession

Erroneous
Convictions
Murder-DNA
Cases % (N)
28 (108)

33 (131)

Erroneous
Convictions Sexual
Assault Cases %
(N)
11 (184)

Near Misses
Murder Cases %
(N)

Near Misses
Sexual Assault
Cases % (N)
6 (67)

Non-eyewitness gave
evidence
Eyewitness misidentification
(unintentional, only cases
with an ID)

51 (109)***

26 (129)***

29 (185)***

9 (67)***

57 (67)

61 (86)

90 (158)***

54 (59)***

Additional misidentification

9 (109)

4 (131)

23 (185)

14 (66)

Intentional misidentification
or lie by victim

N/A

N/A

3 (185)***

33 (67)***

Intentional misidentification
by non-victim eyewitness

26 (109)

33 (131)

3 (185)*

9 (67)*

Intentional misidentification

26 (109)

34 (131)

5 (185)***

40 (67)***

Forensic evidence errors (all
cases)

31 (109)

19 (131)

36 (185)

13 (67)

Forensic evidence errors
(only cases with forensics)

44 (77)*

28 (89)

41 (162)**

17 (52)

Presented w/ no errors

22 (109)

12 (128)

16 (183)***

3 (66)**

Errors

2 (109)

2 (128)

1 (183)***

2 (66)***

Presented w/ no errors

1 (109)***

19 (127)***

N/A

N/A

Errors

2 (109)***

0 (127)***

N/A

N/A

Presented w/ no errors

21 (108)***

3 (128)***

26 (183)***

2 (66)***

Errors

5 (108)***

2 (128)***

7 (183)***

3 (66)***

22 (109)***

2 (128)***

36 (184)***

8 (66)***

Fingerprint

Ballistics

Microscopic hair

Serology
Presented w/ no errors

148
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Errors in testing

2 (109)***

1 (128)***

3 (184)***

0 (66)***

Errors in testimony

12 (109)***

1 (128)***

20 (184)***

2 (66)***

Errors in testing &

2 (109)***

0 (128)***

1 (184)

0 (66)

Presented w/ no errors

6 (109)***

23 (128)***

3 (184)***

42 (66)***

Errors

0 (109)***

2 (128)***

2 (184)***

2 (66)***

3 (109)

2 (131)

1 (185)***

12 (67)***

Any victim recanted crime

N/A

N/A

0 (185)***

24 (67)***

Any victim recanted
identification

N/A

N/A

1 (185)***

28 (67)***

N/A

N/A

91 (149)

88 (57)

N/A

N/A

57 (97)***

21 (48)***

N/A

N/A

34 (101)**

60 (48)**

N/A

N/A

58 (95)

39 (31)

N/A

N/A

21 (118)***

63 (46)***

Show-up

N/A

N/A

9 (118)***

4 (46)***

Photo array

N/A

N/A

58 (118)***

30 (46)***

Lineup

N/A

N/A

11 (118)***

2 (46)***

Single photo

N/A

N/A

2 (118)***

0 (46)***

60 (107)

63 (118)

24 (183)

21 (67)

5 (62)

0 (72)

9 (45)

0 (14)

Testimony
DNA

Surveillance/wiretap
evidence

Victim 1 misidentified the
defendant (only cases where
victim is alive)
Discrepancy b/w victim 1’s
description and defendant
Victim 1 provided unique
details of perp
Victim 1 made cross-racial
ID
Victim 1 identification
method
Direct

Eyewitness 1 misidentified
defendant
Eyewitness 1 took multiple
tries to ID defendant

Strength of witness
characteristics

149
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

18 (109)***

1 (106)***

8 (86)

10 (29)

Probative:

54 (109)

45 (106)

41 (86)

28 (29)

Highly probative:

29 (109)

54 (106)

51 (86)

62 (29)

Strength of identification
information
Weak:

16 (83)

19 (99)

18 (171)*

19 (62)*

Probative:

66 (83)

50 (99)

58 (171)

36 (62)

Highly probative:

18 (83)

31 (99)

25 (171)

45 (62)

Strength of prosecution’s
case
Weak:

51 (109)***

24 (131)***

28 (185)

24 (67)

Probative:

41 (109)

44 (131)

59 (185)

54 (67)

Highly probative:

8 (109)

33 (131)

14 (185)

22 (67)

Weak:

Misleading circumstantial
evidence†
Any non-victim eyewitness
recanted†
Any witness recanted crime
or ID†
Discrepancy b/w any
victim’s description and
defendant†
* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

† no control applied

150
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 20. Bivariate Results: Nature of the Facts Available to the State, Significant
Variables Controlled for Pre- and Post-DNA Resolution
Erroneous
Convictions
Pre-DNA Cases
%(N)
10 (147)

Near Misses
Pre-DNA Cases
% (N)

Forensic evidence errors (all
cases)
Forensic evidence error (only
cases w/ forensics)

Near Misses
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)

12 (2)

Erroneous
Convictions
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)
19 (112)

33 (147)

21 (28)

35 (111)***

14 (170)

39 (122)

25 (20)

48 (81)

21 (113)

Presented w/ no errors

15 (146)

11 (28)

21 (112)**

8 (168)

Errors

1 (146)

7 (28)

1 (112)

1 (168)

1 (147)***

14 (28)

0 (112)***

13 (167)

1 (147)

0 (28)

0 (112)

0 (167)

Presented w/ no errors

20 (147)

11 (2)

20 (168)***

1 (111)

Errors

6 (147)

4 (28)

5 (168)

1 (11)

34 (147)**

11 (28)

21 (112)***

2 (168)

Errors in testing

3 (147)

0 (28)

4 (112)

1 (168)

Errors in testimony

18 (147)

4 (28)

13 (112)

0 (168)

Errors in testing &

1 (147)

0 (28)

3 (112)

0 (168)

Presented w/ no errors

N/A

N/A

7 (112)***

29 (168)

Errors

N/A

N/A

3 (112)

2 (168)

Nature of the Facts Variables

False confession

23 (172)

Fingerprint

Ballistics
Presented w/ no errors
Errors
Microscopic hair

Serology
Presented w/ no errors

Testimony
DNA

Strength of witness

151
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

characteristics
Weak:

15 (76)

0 (22)

10 (73)**

4 (114)**

Probative:

33 (76)

32 (22)

53 (73)

41 (114)

Highly probative:

53 (76)

68 (22)

37 (73)

55 (114)

Weak:

17 (132)*

11 (27)*

20 (100)

20 (139)

Probative:

60 (132)

44 (27)

61 (100)

45 (139)

Highly probative:

23 (132)

44 (27)

19 (100)

35 (139)

Weak:

30 (148)*

18 (28)*

41 (112)***

24 (172)***

Probative:

59 (148)

54 (28)

46 (112)

44 (172)

Highly probative:

11 (148)

29 (28)

13 (112)

31 (172)

Strength of identification
information

Strength of prosecution’s case

Surveillance/wiretap
evidence†
Misleading circumstantial
evidence†
Any victim recanted crime†
Any victim recanted
identification†
Any non-victim eyewitness
recanted†
Any witness recanted crime or
ID†
Victim 1 misidentified the
defendant (victim is alive)†
Discrepancy b/w victim 1’s
description and defendant†
Discrepancy b/w any victim’s
description and defendant†
Victim 1 provided unique
details of perp†
Victim 1 made cross-racial
ID†

152
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Victim 1 identification
method†
Eyewitness 1 misidentified
defendant†
Eyewitness 1 took multiple
tries to ID defendant†
Non-eyewitness testified†
Eyewitness misidentification†
Additional misidentification†
Intentional misidentification or
lie by victim†
Intentional misidentification
by non-victim eyewitness†
Intentional misidentification†
* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

† no control applied

153
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 21. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Quality of Defense: Significant Variables
Controlled for Murder and Sexual Assault
Erroneous Convictions
Murder Cases
%(N)

Near Misses
Murder Cases
% (N)

Erroneous
Convictions
Sexual Assault
Cases
%(N)

Near Misses
Sexual Assault
Cases
%(N)

Defense presented DNA
evidence

0 (81)

4 (102)

1 (136)***

11 (55)***

Defense presented evidence
of misconduct

12 (81)

23 (106)

7 (136)**

24 (55)**

Defense presented
exculpatory evidence

54 (80)*

71 (103)*

70 (139)

73 (55)

Defense presented family
member as a witness

41 (74)*

25 (99)*

58 (127)***

23 (52)***

Quality of Defense Variables

* p<0.05

** p<0.01

*** p<0.001

154
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 22. Bivariate Results: All Variables, Quality of Defense: Significant Variables
Controlled by Pre-DNA and Post-DNA Testing
Erroneous Convictions
Pre-DNA Cases
%(N)

Near Misses
Pre-DNA Cases
% (N)

Erroneous
Convictions
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)

Near Misses
Post-DNA Cases
%(N)

11 (114)

20 (21)

10 (79)*

24 (136)*

NA

NA

3 (79)

5 (138)

Defense presented evidence
of misconduct

6 (114)

14 (21)

8 (79)**

21 (142)**

Defense presented
exculpatory evidence

72 (118)

59 (22)

56 (78)**

75 (139)**

60 (106)**

26 (19)**

40 (75)*

25 (134)*

4 (115)

15 (15)

5 (75)***

26 (133)***

Quality of Defense Variables

Defense presented alternative
suspect
Defense presented DNA
evidence

Defense presented family
member as a witness
Defense presented physical
evidence corroborating alibi
* p<0.05

** p<0.01

***p<0.001

155
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 23. Variables Included After Bivariate Controls
 

Quality of
Work by
Criminal
Justice
Officials

Nature of Facts

Nature of
Crime

Nature of
Defendant

Location
Effects

Category

Variable

Controlled for State

Controlled for Time

Death Penalty Culture

Sig

Sig

Controlled for
Crime
N/A

Crime Consistency

Sig

No

N/A

Age

N/A

N/A

Sig

Criminal History

N/A

Sig

Sig

Race (% African
American)
High School Grad

N/A

No

No

N/A

No

No

White female victim

N/A

N/A

Sig

Female Victim

N/A

N/A

No

High Profile Case

N/A

N/A

No

Strength of Pros. Case

N/A

Sig

No

Forensic Error

N/A

No

Sig

Non-Eyewitness
Evidence
Testimony
Discrepancy
Intentional MisID

N/A

N/A

Sig

N/A

N/A

Sig

N/A

N/A

No

N/A

No

No

N/A

N/A

N/A

N/A

Sig

Sig

N/A

Sig

N/A

N/A

No

N/A

N/A

No

No

N/A

Sig

Sig

Pros. Withheld
Evidence
Time from Arrest to
Indict
Strength of Defense

Quality of Defense

Physical Alibi
Other Suspect
Evidence of
Misconduct
Family Witness

156
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 24. Logistic Regression Results: Nature of the Crime and Location Effect Model
 
Concept

Variable

Coef.

Std. Err.

Location Effects

Death Penalty Culture

146.103***

(43.463)

Crime Consistency

2.113**

(0.821)

Female Victim

0.896**

(0.380)

High Profile Case

-0.433

(0.427)

Nature of the Crime

Controls

Illinois Cases

Num. of Obs. = 460

0.953***

(0.339)

Post DNA

-1.658***

(0.309)

Murder Cases

-0.464

(0.297)

Constant

-2.648*

(1.529)

Num. of Imputations = 5

Area Under ROC Curve = 0.816

Coefficient estimates followed by robust standard errors in parentheses clustered by state.
*** p<0.01, **p<0.05, *p<0.10

157
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 25. Logistic Regression Results: Nature of the Defendant Model
 
Concept

Variable

Coef.

Std. Err.

Nature of the Defendant

Age

-0.031*

(0.018)

Black Defendant

0.153

(0.387)

High School Grad

-0.406

(0.390)

Prior Criminal History

0.880***

(0.192)

Illinois Cases

0.427

(0.264)

Post DNA

-1.863***

(0.282)

Murder Cases

-0.917***

(0.267)

Constant

2.488***

(0.625)

Num. of Imputations = 5

Area Under ROC Curve = 0.801

Controls

Num. of Obs. = 460

Coefficient estimates followed by robust standard errors in parentheses clustered by state.
*** p<0.01, **p<0.05, *p<0.10

158
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 26. Logistic Regression Results: Nature of the Facts Model
 
Concept

Variable

Coef.

Std. Err.

Nature of the Facts

Strength of Pros. Case

-0.974***

(0.366)

Forensic Error

1.012***

(0.324)

Non-eyewitness Testimony/Evidence

0.981***

(0.286)

Testimony Discrepancy

0.517

(0.459)

Unique Perpetrator Description

-0.100

(0.376)

Intentional MisID

-0.605

(0.444)

Illinois Cases

0.521**

(0.254)

Post DNA

-1.877***

(0.232)

Murder Cases

-0.931***

(0.331)

Constant

1.761***

(0.358)

Num. of Imputations = 5

Area Under ROC Curve = 0.827

Controls

Num. of Obs. = 460

Coefficient estimates followed by robust standard errors in parentheses clustered by state.
*** p<0.01, **p<0.05, *p<0.10

159
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 27. Logistic Regression Results: Quality of the Criminal Justice System Model
 
Concept

Variable

Coef.

Std. Err.

Quality of Work by CJ System

Pros. Withheld Evidence

1.483**

(0.655)

Non Eyewitness Lying

1.144***

(0.336)

Time from Arrest to Indict

0.797

(0.494)

Illinois Cases

0.632**

(0.265)

Post DNA

-1.956***

(0.278)

Murder Cases

-1.069***

(0.354)

Controls

Constant
Num. of Obs. = 460

Num. of Imputations = 5

0.453

(0.921)

Area Under ROC Curve =0.811

Coefficient estimates followed by robust standard errors in parentheses clustered by state.
*** p<0.01, **p<0.05, *p<0.10

160
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 28. Logistic Regression Results: Quality of Defense Model
 
Concept

Variable

Quality of Defense

Controls

Num. of Obs. = 460

Coef.

Std. Err.

Strength of Defense Case

-0.902**

(0.371)

Physical Alibi

-1.166**

(0.432)

Alternative Suspect

-0.565

(0.351)

Evidence of Misconduct

-0.612

(0.408)

Family Witness

0.987***

(0.256)

Illinois Cases

0.549**

(0.276)

Post DNA

-1.722***

(0.286)

Murder Cases

-0.778***

(0.288)

Constant

2.187***

(0.364)

Num. of Imputations = 5

Area Under ROC Curve =0.825

Coefficient estimates followed by robust standard errors in parentheses clustered by state.
*** p<0.01, **p<0.05, *p<0.10

161
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 29. Factors that Influence the Likelihood of an Erroneous Conviction
 
Concept

Variable

Nature of the Crime

Death Penalty Culture

Nature of the Defendant

(57.809)

Crime Consistency

1.086

(0.792)

Female Victim

0.601

(0.463)

High Profile Case

-0.715

(0.486)

Age

-0.055**

(0.027)

0.213

(0.383)

-0.309

(0.483)

High School Grad
Prior Criminal History

Strength of Pros. Case

Quality of Defense

0.850***

(0.296)

-1.091**

(0.490)

Forensic Error

0.956**

(0.467)

Non-eyewitness Evidence

0.333

(0.461)

Testimony Discrepancy

0.422

(0.472)

Unique Perpetrator Description

0.270

(0.480)

-0.890**

(0.448)

Intentional MisID

Quality of Work by CJ System

(Std. Err.)

172.848**

Black Defendant

Nature of the Facts

Coef.

Pros. Withheld Evidence

1.655***

(0.557)

Non Eyewitness Lying

1.159**

(0.574)

Time from Arrest to Indict

0.241

(0.493)

Strength of Defense Case

-1.043**

(0.470)

Physical Alibi

-0.716

(0.489)

Other Suspect

-0.693

(0.534)

Evidence of Misconduct

-0.989*

(0.488)

162
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Controls

Num. of Obs. = 460

Family Witness

0.887***

(0.290)

Illinois Cases

0.953**

(0.419)

Post DNA

-1.213***

(0.347)

Murder Cases

-0.674*

(0.364)

Constant

-0.131

(2.111)

Num. of Imputations = 5

Area Under ROC Curve = 0.908

Coefficient estimates followed by robust standard errors in parentheses clustered by state.
*** p<0.01, **p<0.05, *p<0.10

163
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Figure 2. Sample Receiver-Operating Characteristic Curve, Model Includes Nature of the
Crime and Controls
 

8
c:i

0.00

0.25

0.50

0.75

1.00

1 - Specificity
Area under ROC curve =0.8160

164
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Figure 3. Receiver-Operating Characteristic Curve for First Imputed Data Set, Final
Model
 

0 .00

0.25

Area under ROC curve

'=

0.50
1 • Specificity

0.75

1.00

0.908

165
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Figure 4. Probability of an Erroneous Conviction

 
1000 Simulations performed on the 5 Imputed data sets using Clarify (King et al. 2000). Plotted using plotfds
(Boehmke 2008).

166
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 30. Number of Complete Cases, Imputed Cases, and Models used for Imputation
 
Variable

Model

Complete

Incomplete

Imputed

Total

Logged Time to Arrest from

Linear

189

271

271

460

Linear

189

271

271

460

Death Penalty Culture 1

Truncated

358

102

102

460

Death Penalty Culture 2

Truncated

358

102

102

460

Punitive Rank

Truncated

454

6

6

460

Age of Defendant

NBR

446

14

14

460

Black Defendant

Logit

446

14

14

460

High School Grad

Logit

233

227

227

460

Prior Criminal History

Logit

378

82

82

460

Strength of Defense Case

Logit

359

101

101

460

Family Witness

Logit

334

126

126

460

Evidence of Defense

Logit

356

104

104

460

Physical Alibi

Logit

347

113

113

460

Other Suspect

Logit

349

111

111

460

Indictment (Months)
Logged Time to Arrest from
Indictment (Days)

Misconduct

(complete + incomplete = total; imputed is the minimum across m of the number of filled-in
observations.)
 
 

167
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 31. Summary Statistics for Non-imputed Data
 
Variable

Obs

Wrongful Conviction

460

Death Penalty Culture

Std. Dev.

Min

Max

0.565

0.496

0

1

358

0.003

0.003

0.000

0.012

Crime Consistency

460

1.513

0.211

-0.143

2.158

Female Victim

460

0.772

0.420

0

1

Age

446

26.874

8.935

14

76

Black Defendant

446

0.484

0.500

0

1

High School Grad

233

0.536

0.500

0

1

Prior Criminal History

378

0.558

0.497

0

1

Strength of Pros. Case

460

1.202

0.402

0

1

Forensic Error

460

0.252

0.435

0

1

Non-eyewitness Testimony

458

0.266

0.443

0

1

Testimony Discrepancy

460

0.157

0.364

0

1

Unique Perpetrator Description

460

0.167

0.374

0

1

Intentional MisID

460

0.235

0.424

0

1

Crime or ID Recanted

460

0.207

0.405

0

1

Pros. Withheld Evidence

460

0.076

0.265

0

1

Strength of Defense Case

359

0.435

0.496

0

1

Exculpatory Evidence

358

0.687

0.464

0

1

Physical Alibi

347

0.133

0.340

0

1

Other Suspect

349

0.155

0.362

0

1

Illinois Cases

460

0.120

0.325

0

1

Post DNA

460

0.617

0.487

0

1

Murder Cases

460

0.522

0.500

0

1

	

Mean

	
168
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 32. Summary Statistics for Complete vs. Observed Data
 
Complete Data

Observed

5 (Observed and Imputed)
Variable

Obs.

Mean

Std. Err.

Obs.

Mean

Std. Err.

Logged Time to Arrest from

460

0.451

0.262 189

0.452

0.223

460

1.716

0.381 189

1.718

0.321

Death Penalty Culture 1

460

0.045

0.062 358

0.056

0.067

Death Penalty Culture 2

460

0.002

0.003 358

0.003

0.003

Strength of Defense Case

460

0.431

0.496 359

0.435

0.496

Family Witness

460

0.396

0.490 334

0.395

0.490

Physical Alibi

460

0.133

0.340 347

0.133

0.340

Evidence of Defense Misconduct

460

0.124

0.330 356

0.126

0.332

Other Suspect

460

0.154

0.362 349

0.155

0.362

Prior Criminal History

460

0.555

0.498 378

0.558

0.497

Black Defendant

460

0.484

0.500 446

0.484

0.500

Age of Defendant

460

27.000

8.966 446

26.874

8.935

High School Grad

460

0.574

0.495 233

0.536

0.500

Punitive Rank

460

31.965

12.823 454

31.767

12.779

Indictment (Months)
Logged Time to Arrest from
Indictment (Days)

169
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Table 33. Study Results by Hypothesis Proffered
 
Model

Hypothesis

Confirmed?

Traditional Legal
Eyewitness Error
Inadvertent Misidentification
False Confessions
Police or Prosecutor Error
Brady Violations
Tunnel Vision
Forensic Error
Forensic Fraud
Snitch Testimony/Evidence
Lying by any Non-eyewitness
Quality of Defense
Sociological Factors
Race/ethnicity of Defendant
Race/ethnicity of Victim
Cross-racial effects
Age of Defendant
SES of Defendant
Any Criminal History
Similar Criminal History
State Death Penalty Culture
State Political Culture
State Crime Rate

170
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

High Profile Crime
Case Characteristics
Strength of Prosecution Case

*

Multiple Errors
CJ System Actors
Time between Crime and Arrest

* Note, though, that the direction of the relationship is opposite as hypothesized

171
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Figure 5. Probability Density Plots of Observed, Imputed, and Complete Data for Age of
Defendant

172
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Figure 6. Probability Density Plots of Observed, Imputed, and Complete Data for Logged
Time from Indictment to Arrest

173
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Figure 7. Divergent Processes of Innocence Cases

174
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

VIII. APPENDICES

VIII.A. Case Coding Document

1) Name of defendant

2) Is the case in the Innocence Project profiles?

3) Previous criminal conviction?

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

4) Number of prior criminal convictions? (write in)

175
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

5) High school graduate? (write in any additional educational or socioeconomic information that is
available)

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

6) Does the defendant have a history of mental illness or retardation?

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

7) Fluent in English?

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

8) Does the defendant have a history of gang affiliation?

a) Yes
176
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

b) No

c) Unknown

9) Number of co-defendant(s) at trial, if any (write in names if known) and disposition of their cases

10) Number of alleged perpetrators other than the defendant, if any (write in names if known) and
disposition of their cases

11) Is a co-defendant or alleged co-perpetrator guilty of the crime(s) for which the defendant was
charged and/or convicted?

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

d) Not applicable

12) Number of victims involved in:

177
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

a) crime(s) charged________

b) crime(s) convicted_________

13) Race/ethnicity of defendant

a) Caucasian/white

b) African American/black

c) Hispanic/Latino

d) Asian American

e) Native American

f) Other

g) Unknown

178
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

14) Race/ethnicity of victim: (if more than 2 victims, add in)

a) Victim #1:

i) Caucasian/white

ii) African American/black

iii) Hispanic/Latino

iv) Asian American

v) Native American

vi) Other

vii) Unknown

b) Victim #2:

i) Caucasian/white

ii) African American/black

179
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

iii) Hispanic/Latino

iv) Asian American

v) Native American

vi) Other

vii) Unknown

15) Relationship between defendant and victim(s) (if more than 2 victims, add in)

a) Victim #1:

i) Family member/Significant other (specify)

ii) Friend

iii) Acquaintance

iv) Neighbor

180
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

v) Stranger

vi) Other (specify)

vii) Unknown

b) Victim #2:

i) Family member/Significant other (specify)

ii) Friend

iii) Acquaintance

iv) Neighbor

v) Stranger

vi) Other (specify)

vii) Unknown

181
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

16) Age of defendant (if unknown, write that down)

17) Age of victim(s) (if more than 2 victims, add in)

a) Victim #1

b) Victim #2

18) Gender of defendant

a) Male

b) Female

19) Gender of victim(s) (if more than 2 victims, add in)

a) Victim #1:

i) Male

ii) Female

182
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

b) Victim #2:

i) Male

ii) Female

20) Type of crime(s) with which defendant was charged

21) Type of crime(s) for which defendant was convicted

22) Was the alleged offense(s) a serial crime?

23) Date of crime (if unknown, write that down)

24) Date on which defendant was first arrested, charged and/or indicted (if unknown, write that
down)

25) Location/jurisdiction of crime (write in state, county, and city if known)

183
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

26) Law enforcement agency of arrest and investigation (write in)

27) Prosecutor’s office to indict (geographic location) (write in)

28) Name of chief District Attorney/State’s Attorney (write in)

29) Court where case was brought (write in)

30) Date of first trial court resolution (trial/plea/dispositive motion) (write in)

31) Result from first court proceeding:

a) Guilt

b) Acquittal

184
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

c) Dismissal

c) Other (describe)

32) Method of Disposition:

a) By trial

i) Jury trial

ii) Bench or judge trial

b) By plea bargain

c) By Motion

i) On defense motion

ii) On prosecution motion

d) Unknown

185
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

33) If convicted, sentence/penalty:

a) Number of years & months

b) Life sentence

c) Sentenced to death

d) Other

e) Unknown

34) If convicted, was case appealed?

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

35) If yes, in what court was initial appeal heard (write in name and location)?

186
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

36) What was the result?

a) Conviction affirmed (describe court’s rationale)

b) Conviction overturned and case thrown out (describe court’s rationale)

c) Conviction overturned and new trial ordered (describe court’s rationale)

d) Unknown

e) Other (please describe)

37) If conviction was affirmed, did defendant seek further appeals? Write in all further direct
appeals, their dates of resolution, their results, and the courts’ rationales.

38) Did the defendant file any petitions for habeas corpus, whether in state or federal court? Write
in all habeas appeals, their dates of resolution, their results, and the courts’ rationales.

187
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

39) Did the defendant file any motions or petitions asserting an independent claim for relief
available in state court? Write in all independent claims, their dates of resolution, their results, and
the courts’ rationales.

40) Multiple trials to reach guilty verdict?

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

d) No applicable

41) If new trial was ordered, what was the date of its resolution? (write in)

42) Result of that court proceeding (second time):

a) Guilt

b) Acquittal

188
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

c) Dismissal

c) Other (describe)

43) Method of Disposition (second time):

a) By trial

i) Jury trial

ii) Bench or judge trial

b) By plea bargain

c) By Motion

i) On defense motion

ii) On prosecution motion

d) Unknown

189
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

44) If convicted (second time), sentence/penalty:

a) Number of years & months

b) Life sentence

c) Sentenced to death

d) Other

e) Unknown

45) If convicted (second time), was case appealed?

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

46) If second conviction was appealed, in what court was initial appeal heard (write in name and
location)?

190
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

47) What was the result (second appeal)?

a) Conviction affirmed (describe court’s rationale)

b) Conviction overturned and case thrown out (describe court’s rationale)

c) Conviction overturned and new trial ordered (describe court’s rationale)

d) Unknown

e) Other (please describe)

48) If the second conviction was affirmed, did defendant seek further appeals? Write in all further
direct appeals, their results, and the courts’ rationales.

49) Did the defendant file any petitions for habeas corpus (on second conviction), whether in state or
federal court? Write in all habeas appeals, their results and the courts’ rationales.

191
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

50) Did the defendant file any motions or petitions asserting an independent claim for relief
available in state court (on second conviction)? Write in all independent claims, their dates of
resolution, their results, and the courts’ rationales.

51) Type of defense attorney (write in Public Defender, Court Appointed, Pro Bono, Private
Attorney, Pro se, or another description):

a) original proceeding_________________

b) subsequent appeals and/or other proceedings________________________-

52) How do we know defendant was factually innocent (circle as many as apply and briefly list
reason supporting the categories chosen)? (Note: This applies to both convictions and
acquittals/dismissals.)

a) It can be objectively demonstrated that no crime ever occurred

b) It can be objectively demonstrated that it was physically impossible for the defendant to have
committed the crime (if so, please describe why it was physically impossible)

192
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

c) Scientific evidence exonerated the defendant and established his innocence (please list type or
types of scientific evidence)

d) The true perpetrator was identified, apprehended and/or convicted (state how we know why
this person is the true perpetrator)

e) Other (please describe)

53) Source of exoneration (choose only one)

a) Governor’s pardon

b) State trial court overturned conviction with prejudice

c) State appellate court overturned conviction with prejudice

d) Highest state court overturned conviction with prejudice

e) State court of unknown level overturned conviction with prejudice

193
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

f) Federal district court overturned conviction with prejudice

g) Federal circuit court overturned conviction with prejudice

h) U.S. Supreme Court overturned conviction with prejudice

i) State trial court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered, prosecution
dismissed

j) State appellate court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered, prosecution
dismissed

k) Highest state court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered, prosecution
dismissed

l) State court of unknown level overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered,
prosecution dismissed

m) Federal district court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered,
prosecution dismissed

n) Federal circuit court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered, prosecution
dismissed

o) U.S. Supreme Court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered, prosecution
dismissed

p) Other

194
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

54) Did defendant receive a pardon from the governor?

1) Yes

2) No

55) Date of exoneration

56) Individual(s) responsible for defendant’s exoneration (Please put a number beside each category;
a “0” if individual/group is not known to have been involved in the exoneration, a “1” if
individual/group was predominately responsible for the exoneration, or a “2” if individual/group
actively opposed the exoneration; there can be multiple individuals/groups assigned each number)

a) Defendant

b) Victim/supposed victim

c) Real culprit

d) Witness(es)

e) Police

195
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

f) Convicting prosecutor

g) Subsequent prosecutor

h) Judge

i) State administrative official

j) Federal law enforcement

k) Original/trial defense attorney

l) Subsequent defense attorney

m) Family of defendant

n) Friend of defendant

p) Journalist

q) Professor

196
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

r) Innocence project or organization

s) Other (describe)

t) Unknown

57) Evidence presented and source(s) of error – First Indictment or Conviction

a)

Did the victim testify at trial?

1. Yes

2. No

3. Unknown

4. Not applicable

b) Did the victim recant his/her report of the crime?

1. Yes

2. No

3. Unknown

197
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

c) Did the defendant testify at trial?

1. Yes

2. No

3. Unknown

4. Not applicable

d)

Eyewitness identification used?

a) Yes (if yes, continue to fill out this section)

b) No

c) Unknown

1) If yes, did the eyewitness(es) misidentify the defendant?

a. Yes (if yes, continue to fill out this section)

b. No

c. Other (describe)

d. Unknown

198
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

2) Please list number of eyewitnesses misidentifying the defendant

3) Please list who the eyewitnesses were (e.g. victim, bystander, family member)

4) Did the eyewitness provide a description of the perpetrator and if so, was it unique?
Did it differ in a significant way from the defendant’s actual appearance?

5) Was the identification (please circle as many as apply; if more than 2 eyewitness
identifications were made, add in):

a) Eyewitness #1:
i) show-up
ii) line-up
1. sequential
2. simultaneous
3. method unknown
iii) single photo
iv) photo array
1. sequential

199
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

2. simultaneous
3. method unknown
v) directly implicated defendant

b) Eyewitness #2:
i) show-up
ii) line-up
1. sequential
2. simultaneous
3. method unknown
iii) single photo
iv) photo array
1. sequential
2. simultaneous
3. method unknown
v) directly implicated defendant

6) Was the identification cross-racial/ethnic?

a) Yes

b) No

200
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

c) Unknown

7) What was the length of time between the crime and the first positive identification
made by the eyewitness? (if more than two eyewitness identifications were made, add in)

a) Eyewitness #1______________
b) Eyewitness #2______________

8) How certain was the eyewitness of the identification? (if the eyewitness was not
certain, put “0”, if certain, put “1”; if more than two eyewitness identifications were
made, please add in)

a) Eyewitness #1______________
b) Eyewitness #2______________

9) Was there police misconduct or procedural error involved in the misidentification?
(describe)

a) Eyewitness #1____________________
b) Eyewitness #2____________________

10) Did witness testify at trial?

201
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

a) Eyewitness #1____________________
b) Eyewitness #2____________________

11) Was pretrial identification presented at trial?

a) Eyewitness #1____________________
b) Eyewitness #2____________________

12) Did the witness recant? (if yes, write in when the recantation occurred)

a) Eyewitness #1____________________
b) Eyewitness #2____________________

e)

Other eyewitness misidentification?

1) What was misidentified?

2) Who did the identification?

3) How was the identification made?

f)

False confession?

202
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1) Please circle level of incrimination (at what level can the “false confession” be
classified?)

a) false incriminating statement (e.g., defendant saying they were at the crime scene
but saying nothing else)

b) false admission/false confession (e.g., defendant says “I did it” or gives
explanation of how or why he/she committed the crime)

2) Please list conditions under which confession was elicited (e.g., use of physical
violence or threats)

g)

Perjury or lying/fabrication by witness?

1) Please list number of witnesses

2) Please list type of witness(es) (e.g., Jailhouse informant or snitch testimony)

h) Perjury or lying/fabrication by criminal justice official(s) (list number and type of officials)?

203
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

i)

Other unreliable or mistaken prosecution testimony (i.e., not eyewitness testimony and not

perjured/fabricated testimony)?

j)

Forensic evidence presented at trial?

a) Yes

b) No

c) Unknown

1) If yes, write in type(s) of forensic evidence presented and errors that occurred

2) Was there fraud?

k)

Withholding of exculpatory evidence (i.e., evidence tending to show the defendant was

innocent); list type of evidence withheld and individual withholding the evidence?

l)

Police error (please describe)?

m) Police misconduct (please describe)?
204
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

n) Prosecutorial error (please describe)?

o)

Prosecutorial misconduct (please describe)?

p) Judicial error (please describe)?

q)

Judicial misconduct (please describe)?

r)

Juror error (please describe)?

s)

Juror misconduct (please describe)?

t)

Error or misconduct by other criminal justice officials (please describe)?

205
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

u)

Incompetent defense counsel?

v) Miscellaneous

1) Misleading circumstantial evidence?

2) Erroneous judgment on cause of death?

3) Community pressure?

4) Other errors (list as descriptively as possible)

w) Did the defense case present the following evidence (circle as many as apply and briefly note
the strength of the evidence presented)?

1) Alibi evidence/witnesses

2) Forensic evidence

3) Eyewitness(es) of crime

4) Misconduct of criminal justice officials

5) Evidence incriminating another individual

206
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

6) Other (describe)

58) Evidence presented and source(s) of error – Second Indictment or Conviction
[omitted: questions are repeated from #57]

59) What was the length of time:

a) served in prison from conviction/charge to release? (write in any correctional control imposed
on defendant after release)

1. Years and months _____________
2. Unknown

b) between the conviction/charge and the point at which the defendant finally exited the criminal
justice system with regards to the crime at issue?

1. Years and months _____________
2. Unknown

60) Did the defendant receive compensation?

a) Yes

(write in amount_______________)
207
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

b) No

61) If yes, what was the compensation for and how and when was it granted (write in)?

62) Where is the defendant now? (write in)

CONTACT INFORMATION:
(Please provide name, connection to case, contact information, and method of contact for each
individual from which information was obtained about this case)

SOURCE INFORMATION:
(Please provide type of source and citation for each source consulted about this case)

208
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

VIII.B. Coding Book (for Case Coding Document)

1) Name of defendant (last name, first name, middle name):
This is the full legal name used on court documents.

2) Is the case in the Innocence Project profiles?
This refers to whether or not the defendant has a profile on the Innocence Project website.

3) Previous criminal conviction?
This refers to prior convictions before the time of arrest. Answer “Yes,” “No,” or “Unknown”
and write in an explanation if needed. If known, write in the type of conviction (felony or
misdemeanor) and charge (i.e., burglary). This includes any convictions in any jurisdiction in the
United States. Also, please make a note if the defendant has previously been arrested or charged,
but not convicted.

4) Number of prior criminal convictions (write in):
Be as precise as possible, but if the actual number is not known, please make an estimate (i.e.,
only a few, several, many).

5) High school graduate?
This refers to level of education obtained prior to arrest. Answer “Yes,” “No,” or “Unknown”.
Do not assume an answer unless there are strong indications one way or the other. Write in any

209
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

additional educational or socioeconomic information that is available, including highest level of
education received, previous employment experience, and previous homelessness.

6) Does the defendant have a history of mental illness or retardation?
This is an objective standard, so do not consider whether the impairment or disability was known
at the time the defendant was arrested and/or brought to trial. However, only impairments or
disabilities that existed at the time of the crime or the defendant’s indictment should be noted
(i.e., disabilities developed after conviction, dismissal, or acquittal are excluded). This question
is meant to be as inclusive as possible and includes emotional and personality disorders as well
as chemical addictions that affect behavior or memory loss. In addition, if a defendant is of low
intelligence/has an IQ below 80 (WAIS-IV), mark “Yes.” If there is no indication that the
defendant had a mental disorder, you may assume “No.”

7) Fluent in English?
Answer “Yes,” “No,” or “Unknown”. Choose “No” if there is any indication that defendant was
not conversant or could not write in English. If there is no reasonable expectation that the
defendant spoke another language, choose “Yes.” “Unknown” should only be chosen when there
is a possibility that the defendant was not fluent in English, but you are not certain.

8) Does the defendant have a history of gang affiliation?
This refers to the defendant only. If no mention is made of possible gang affiliation, select “No.”
By itself, participating in the sale of narcotics is NOT enough to indicate gang affiliation.

210
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

9) Number of co-defendant(s) at trial, if any (write in names if known) and disposition of
their cases:
Co-defendant is used in the legal sense, as an individual indicted for the same crime and not
subsequently severed at trial. An individual is still considered a co-defendant if (s)he did not go
to trial with the defendant because one or the other disposed of his/her case through a plea. Make
a note if the co-defendant confessed or implicated the defendant.

10) Number of perpetrators other than the defendant, if any (write in names if known) and
disposition of their cases:
This is the number and name of any perpetrators other than the defendant but including codefendants, who the police or prosecution alleges participated in the crime(s) for which the
defendant was convicted. Please briefly indicate whether the co-perpetrators were ever arrested
and charged, and if so, what were the dispositions of their cases. Make a note if the coperpetrator confessed or implicated the defendant.

11) Is a co-defendant or alleged co-perpetrator guilty of the crime(s) for which the
defendant was charged and/or convicted?
Answer “Yes” if the co-defendant or co-perpetrator was convicted (or pled guilty) and was never
exonerated. Answer “No” if a co-defendant or co-perpetrator was found guilty and later
exonerated. Answer “Not applicable” if there were no co-defendants or alleged co-perpetrators.

211
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

12) Number of victims:
Crimes charged: This is the number of victims involved in crime(s) for which the defendant was
charged together in the relevant jurisdiction.
Crimes convicted: This is the number of victims involved in crime(s) of which the defendant was
convicted together in the relevant jurisdiction.

13) Race/ethnicity of defendant:
Use the race/ethnicity designated either by official record or by colloquial usage; if defendant is
of mixed race, designate “other” and write in “mixed.”

14) Race/ethnicity of victim(s):
Use the race/ethnicity designated either by official record or by colloquial usage; if victim is of
mixed race, designate “other” and write in “mixed.” If there is more than one victim, make a
notation for the race of each victim even if they are of the same race.

15) Relationship between defendant and victim(s):
“Neighbor” includes anyone whose physical living proximity to the victim is noted in official
reports or the media (i.e., said to have been living a few streets down, in an adjacent apartment
complex, etc.). Family member/significant other includes extended family by birth or marriage,
as well as any close romantic relationship. If, from the available facts, it is not clear what the
relationship is between the defendant and victim(s), designate “unknown” rather than “stranger.”
If there is more than one victim, make a notation for the relationship of each victim even if they
have the same relationship.

212
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

16) Age of defendant:
This refers to the age of the defendant at the time of the offense. If there are a range of dates, use
the mean.

17) Age of victim(s):
This refers to the age of the victim(s) at the time of the offense. If there is more than one victim,
make a notation for the age of each victim even if they are the same age.

18) Gender of defendant:

19) Gender of victim(s):
If there is more than one victim, make a notation for the gender of each victim even if they are
the same gender.

20) Type of crime(s) with which defendant was charged:
Include the number of counts if more than one.

21) Type of crime(s) for which defendant was convicted:
Include the number of counts if more than one. If we know that the defendant was convicted of a
lesser included offense, please designate that. If the defendant was convicted of any offense that
the judge then set aside (due to juvenile status, etc.), include this offense but make a note that it
was set aside.

213
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

22) Was the alleged offense(s) a serial crime?
Answer “Yes” if police or prosecutors think the crime at issue was part of a serial crime event.
Please write in whether this was known at the time that the defendant was originally charged or
convicted. A serial murder is defined as: “the unlawful killing of two or more victims by the
same offender(s), in separate events.” [FBI Serial Murder Report] A serial rape is similarly
defined as: “the unlawful sexual assault of two or more victims by the same offender(s), in
separate events.”

23) Date of crime (if unknown, write that down)
Include day, month, and year if known.

24) Date on which defendant was first arrested, charged and/or indicted (if unknown, write
that down):
If the dates of arrest, charge, and/or indictment differ, please designate which date is which. If
you do not have any other relevant dates, you may include the date of arraignment, but please
make a note of this.

25) Location/jurisdiction of crime (write in state, county, and city if known):

26) Law enforcement agency of arrest and investigation (write in):
This should be the agency that took responsibility for bringing the defendant to prosecution (e.g.,
Arlington County Sheriff’s Office). If the agency had only a minor, brief role in the case, such as
arresting the defendant and handing him over to another agency, or providing information about

214
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

a suspect, do not include it. You may include more than one agency if substantive investigation
was performed by multiple agencies.

27) Prosecutor’s office to indict (geographic location) (write in):
This is the official name of the prosecutor’s office as well as jurisdictional or geographic location
if unclear from the name of the office.

28) Name of chief District Attorney/State’s Attorney:
Use the name of the individual in office when the defendant was first convicted. If there is more
than one trial and the chief DA or SA is different, note this.

29) Court where case was brought (write in):
If the case was removed or changed venue please note this. This may not be relevant question to
for the dismissals.

30) Date of trial court resolution (trial/plea/dispositive motion) (write in):
Include day, month, and year if known. Do not use the sentencing date.

31) Result from first court proceeding:
Choose from “guilty,” “acquittal,” “dismissal,” and “other.” “Other” includes hung juries and
mistrials. If there is more than one disposition, e.g., the defendant was acquitted of one charge
and found guilty of another, please indicate this by circling all that apply and writing in an
explanation. If the defendant is found guilty of a lesser included offense but not the primary

215
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

offense, indicate this as “guilty” (remember, there is an earlier question that asks what offenses
the defendant was convicted of).

32) Method of disposition:
If there is more than one disposition, e.g., the defendant pled guilty for one offense but stood trial
for another, indicate this by circling all that apply and writing in an explanation.

33) If convicted, sentence/penalty:
If more than one category applies because defendant was convicted of multiple crimes, fill in all
that apply. If more than one life sentence is imposed, write that in, as well as if the life sentence
is without the possibility of parole.

34) If convicted, was case appealed?

35) If yes, in what court was initial appeal heard (write in name and location):

36) What was the result?
If there is more than one disposition, e.g., the court affirms the conviction on one charge but
orders a new trial for another charge, please indicate this by circling all that apply and writing in
an explanation.
If the outcome was positive for the defendant and the prosecution appealed, write this in and
explain the outcome of the prosecution’s appeal. This will help explain why the defendant might
have pursued further appeals despite an initial positive outcome of the initial appeal.

216
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

37) If conviction was affirmed, did the defendant seek further appeals? Write in all further
direct appeals, their dates of resolution, their results, and the courts’ rationales.
Indicate the name of the court hearing the appeal and the level of the court (e.g., Court of
Appeals in a jurisdiction may designate an intermediate court or a final court of appeals). Make
sure to distinguish between a discretionary appeal that the court refused to hear and an appeal
that the court agreed to hear but the defendant lost.

38) Did the defendant file any petitions for habeas corpus, whether in state or federal
court? Write in all habeas appeals, their dates of resolution, their results, and the courts’
rationales.
Note that this question will not be used in further quantitative analysis so please do not make this
a priority in your research. Make sure to indicate whether the petitions were filed in state or
federal court and at which level.

39) Did the defendant file any motions or petitions asserting an independent claim for
relief available in state court? Write in all independent claims, their dates of resolution,
their results, and the courts’ rationales.
Note that this question will not be used in further quantitative analysis so please do not make this
a priority in your research. Independent claims include state remedies other than direct appeals or
habeas petitions, including a writ of actual innocence (as in Virginia) or in some jurisdictions, a
motion for a new trial based on new evidence.

217
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

40) Multiple trials to reach guilty verdict?
Indicate “Yes” if initial trial(s) resulted in no verdict (i.e., a hung jury or a mistrial) and a
subsequent trial led to a guilty verdict. Select “No” if there was only one trial or if there were
multiple trials with a guilty verdict each time. Select “Unknown” if there was a guilty verdict
(i.e., it is an erroneous conviction case) but you are uncertain about how many trials occurred.
Select “Not applicable” if it is a rightful acquittal/dismissal case.

41) If a new trial was ordered, what was the date of its resolution? (write in)
If a new trial was ordered, but the prosecutor declines to re-prosecute, write in the date of this
decision.

42) Result of that court proceeding (second time):
See instructions for Q 31, above. If the second court proceeding resulted in a third proceeding
(e.g., another mistrial resulted in a third trial), please indicate this and add the information in Qs
37-46 for the third court proceeding. Continue this process for any additional court proceedings.

43) Method of disposition (second time):
See Q 32, above.

44) If convicted (second time), sentence/penalty:
See instructions for Q 33, above.

218
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

45) If convicted (second time), was case appealed?
See instructions for Q 34, above .

46) If second conviction was appealed, in what court was initial appeal heard (write in
name and location)?
See instructions for Q 35, above .

47) What was the result (second appeal)?
See instructions for Q 36, above.

48) If the second conviction was affirmed, did defendant seek further appeals? Write in all
further direct appeals, their results, and the courts’ rationales.
See instructions for Q 37, above.

49) Did the defendant file any petitions for habeas corpus (on second conviction), whether
in state or federal court? Write in all habeas appeals, their results and the courts’
rationales.
See instructions for Q 38, above.

50) Did the defendant file any motions or petitions asserting an independent claim for relief
available in state court (on second conviction)? Write in all independent claims, their dates
of resolution, their results, and the courts’ rationales.
See instructions for Q 39 above.

219
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

51) Type of defense attorney (write in Public Defender, Court Appointed, Pro Bono,
Private Attorney, Pro Se, or another description): original proceeding

subsequent

appeals and/or other proceedings
This question only includes defense attorneys who actually represented the defendant (e.g. they
formed an attorney-client relationship). Any defense attorney who represented the defendant
before or at the time of sentencing should be included in “original proceeding.” If the attorney
was involved in a subsequent stage, note what stage this was (i.e., habeas petition, executive
clemency petition, etc.). Write in if there were multiple types of defense attorneys representing
the defendant during either period (you do not need to note if there were two different
individuals, i.e., two public defenders, if this information is not significant for another reason). If
the reason that the type of defense attorney changed is significant, please note this.

52) How do we know defendant was factually innocent (circle as many as apply and briefly
list reason supporting the categories chosen)?
Include evidence cited by officials in their decision to exonerate or dismiss the defendant, as well
as any additional information that you may have collected from research that indicates the
defendant’s innocence.

53) Source of exoneration (choose only one):
You do not need to answer this question for the acquittals/dismissals.
This question is referring to the legal act that finally dissolved the defendant’s responsibility for
the crimes with which he was convicted. Thus, choose (a) “Governor’s pardon” only if this was
the first act that actually dissolved his responsibility—for instance, if the defendant completed

220
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

his sentence but was still a convicted felon until he received a pardon. If the defendant’s
conviction was overturned by a court but there was no statement of innocence until he received a
pardon, the exoneration is still the overturning of the conviction by the court and the subsequent
dismissal of charges (see Q 54 for a place to designate that he also received a pardon). Choose
“other” if the conviction was overturned by a court, a new trial was ordered, and the charges
were eventually dismissed by another court.

Also, write in how the exoneration fulfills our requirement of an official recognition of
innocence. In some cases, the exoneration itself will include such a statement (e.g., a full pardon
based on innocence) but other times the legal exoneration and the recognition of innocence may
not be simultaneous. If this is the case, next to the legal exoneration make a note of the statement
upon which you are relying (e.g., case was dismissed by prosecutor; later the AG makes an
announcement that the state has found the real killers and are indicting two other men.)

54) Did defendant receive a pardon?

55) Date of exoneration:
For the acquittals/dismissals, this will be the same date as you indicated in Q 30.
If the exoneration occurred through a governor’s pardon, use the date of the pardon. If the
exoneration occurred through legislative action, use the date the bill was passed. If the
exoneration occurred through a court declaration of innocence, or an overturned or set aside
conviction, use the date of the court’s opinion or order. If the exoneration occurred through other
court action, coupled with a prosecutor’s decision to dismiss charges or vacate the conviction,

221
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

use the last date at which this action was given legal effect (i.e., the date that the court grants the
motion to vacate or the date that the prosecutor dismisses the charges).

56) Individual(s) responsible for defendant’s exoneration:
You do not need to answer this question for the acquittals/dismissals.
Put a “1” next to any individual/group that was predominately responsible for the exoneration.
Thos e that are predominately responsible include individuals or groups who are “but for” causes
of the exoneration or those who played crucial, direct, and active roles in the exoneration (e.g.,
an agency that takes the initiative to retest forensic evidence).

Put a “2” next to any individual/group who actively opposed the exoneration, particularly in the
face of overwhelming evidence of innocence. For instance, the prosecutor’s office should receive
a “2” if the office maintained that the defendant was guilty despite an official recognition of
innocence or actively sought to hinder the exoneration in some way.

Prosecutor refers to both the individual and the office (if there is a discrepancy between the roles
of the individual prosecutor and the office in the exoneration, indicate this). Police refers to city,
county, or state police, as distinguished from federal law enforcement. An innocence project or
organization must be an organization whose purpose is to establish the innocence of defendants
who are erroneously charged or convicted of a crime.

57) Evidence presented and source(s) of error – First Indictment or Conviction

222
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

a) Did the victim testify at trial?
Answer “Yes,” “No,” “Unknown,” or “Not applicable.” This includes any type of testimony.
Select “Not applicable” if there was no trial (i.e., the case is a dismissal or plea) or if the victim
is deceased.

b) Did the victim recant his/her report of the crime?
Indicate whether the victim, after reporting the crime to the police, recanted his or her statement
that a crime occurred. If known, write in when the recantation happened (i.e., before, during, or
after trial). This does NOT include a recantation of eyewitness identification, see (d). You may
assume “No” if there was no indication to the contrary.

c) Did the defendant testify at trial?
If you have information about the nature of the defendant’s testimony, write this in. Select “Not
applicable” if there was no trial (i.e., the case is a dismissal or plea).

d) Eyewitness identification
First designate whether eyewitness identification was used in the investigation and prosecution
of the crime. This includes any informal or formal identification procedure used by law
enforcement or prosecution, including pre-trial procedures. Eyewitnesses include individuals
who witnessed the defendant commit the crime as well as those who identify the defendant as
someone who was at or near the scene of the crime. It also includes alleged co-perpetrators who
falsely implicated the defendant in the crime (i.e., say they committed the crime together).

223
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1) Next, designate whether any eyewitness misidentified the defendant as the perpetrator or the
person at or near the scene during an identification procedures. This refers to either mistaken
identification or perjured testimony. However, if an eyewitness perjures herself when identifying
the defendant, this should be noted here and also included under 57 (g) or (h), below.
2- 3) List the number and type of eyewitnesses who misidentified the defendant (this question
and the following questions are no longer concerned with individuals who correctly identified
the defendant).
4) Indicate whether the eyewitness provided a description of the perpetrator. This includes
creating a composite sketch. If the witness provided a description, put down whether the
description was unique. Unique identifiers include tattoos, missing or gold teeth, unusual scars, a
limp or a name. If the witness provided a description, write in whether it differed in a significant
way from the defendant’s actual appearance You may rely on reports that say the description was
different from the defendant’s actual appearance, but only if there are adequate facts to back this
up (in other words, the report should describe what was different about the appearance).
5) Indicate the specific type(s) of identification procedures that were used in the
misidentification, noting the sequence of procedures, as well as whether the eyewitness identified
the defendant in each procedure.
6) Designate whether the misidentification was cross racial/ethnic.
7) Designate the length of time between the crime and the first positive misidentification of the
defendant.
8) Designate the level of certainty in the eyewitness’s misidentification. If any uncertainty was
reported, designate this as “Uncertain”. If multiple procedures were used, please specify the level

224
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

of certainty for each. If the ID procedures are described in full and there is no reason to think
otherwise, you may assume the eyewitness was certain.
9) Note whether police misconduct or procedural error was involved in the identification process.
Please also make a note of whether the existence of misconduct or error was recognized by a
court or criminal justice official or is based upon the opinion of the media or an organization like
the Innocence Project. If you indicate police misconduct or error in the eyewitness ID process,
this should also be indicated under 57 (l) or (m).
10-11) Designate whether the eyewitness testified at trial and whether the pre-trial identification
of each eyewitness was used at trial. If the pre-trial identification was excluded by the court,
please explain why. If there was no trial, i.e., it was a dismissal or plea, then write in “No.” If
you know that the primary evidence against the defendant was eyewitness identification, you
may assume that both pretrial and trial identifications were used.
12) Finally, note whether any eyewitness recanted his or her misidentification before, during, or
after trial. Again, if no mention is made of this, you may assume “No.”

e) Other misidentification This includes an eyewitness who testifies (erroneously) to seeing the
defendant’s car, clothing, etc., at or near the scene of the crime. It also includes identifications
made through senses other than vision, such as recognizing the defendant’s voice or the feel of
his face. As in 57 (d), this can include perjured testimony (such testimony should also be marked
under 57 (g) or (h)). However, it does not include testimony that is correct (i.e., the witness really
did see the defendant near the scene of the crime) but was nonetheless misleading or improperly
used. This type of testimony may be marked under 57 (h) or another subpoint.

225
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

f) False confession If there is police error or misconduct involved in the false confession,
indicate this here, as well as under 57 (l) or (m). A confession is an inculpatory statement given
to a criminal justice official (rather than, for instance, a statement supposedly given to a jailhouse
snitch and subsequently reported to police, which should be marked under (g) below).

g) Perjury or lying/fabrication by witness (other than criminal justice official) Only include
witness testimony where there is clear evidence that the person made deliberately false or
misleading statements, rather than simply being mistaken. This evidence will usually come from
a substantiated admission by the witness that he lied or the prosecution of the witness for perjury;
if the evidence does not rise to this level but there are substantial indications that the witness may
have lied, note that the fabrication is “alleged.”

h) Perjury or lying/fabrication by criminal justice official(s) See (g), above

i) Other unreliable or mistaken prosecution testimony (i.e., not eyewitness testimony and
not perjured/fabricated testimony) This includes mistaken testimony about circumstantial
evidence or motive. It also includes testimony that had certain indicia of unreliability, such as the
testimony of a drug addict or a mentally unstable witness, jealous husband, etc.

j) Forensic evidence presented at trial? Answer “Yes,” “No,” or “Unknown.” If you answer
“Yes,” continue to fill out this question. Forensic evidence is evidence that has been evaluated
using scientific methods.

226
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1) List the types of forensic evidence used (e.g., serology, fingerprints, DNA) and the errors that
occurred in either the testing of the evidence or its presentation at trial. Error does NOT include
lack of testing unless such testing was sanctionable by the court. The presence of error should be
based on available facts and the conclusions of scholars or criminal justice officials. Please make
a note of what the forensic evidence attempted to prove or if it were exculpatory.
2) Indicate whether there was evidence of forensic fraud (e.g., the knowing presentation of
falsified results or the willful presentation of results in a false light).

Please note that for the following variables, you have the option of putting “alleged” as well as
“yes” or “no.” Only select “yes” when an official source, such as the appellate court, governor,
or civil jury, has recognized the error/misconduct, or when it is evident on its face based upon
uncontroverted facts.

k) Withholding of exculpatory evidence (i.e., evidence tending to show the defendant was
innocent); list type of evidence withheld and individual withholding the evidence.
This is essentially Brady violations. It refers to evidence withheld by police or prosecutors. If
information was withheld by a crime lab, psychologist, or other state-affiliated person, do not
include this here—mark it as other criminal justice official error or misconduct, below.

l) Police error This is a mistake or omission that is made by the police and does not imply
intentional wrongdoing. It includes forgetting to collect or losing evidence, suggestive
identification procedures that do not violate a defendant’s rights, and sloppy interrogations where
information is unintentionally leaked to the victim or defendant.

227
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

m) Police misconduct This is an action by the police that violates a defendant’s constitutional
rights. There is an element of intentionality or extreme negligence that is either present or can be
legally inferred. It includes Miranda violations, physical abuse of the defendant, or the
fabrication of evidence. It only includes a Brady violation, see (k), when there is evidence of
intentionality or extreme negligence in the withholding. It also includes coercive interrogation
tactics where the defendant later wins a civil suit against the officers. Please note, however, that
police are usually allowed to lie to defendants in interrogation or use other forms of
psychological pressure. Therefore, do not mark this behavior as error or misconduct unless there
is strong evidence to indicate that it was deemed unacceptable (e.g., a mentally handicapped
defendant gets his conviction overturned based on a coerced confession).

n-t) Repeat designation of error and misconduct for prosecution, judge, juror, or other
criminal justice official

u) Incompetent defense counsel? Ideally, this will be based on an official court finding of
ineffective assistance of counsel. However, you may also answer “Yes” if the behavior was
incompetent on its face (e.g., forgetting to file court documents).

v) Miscellaneous
1) Misleading circumstantial evidence: Circumstantial evidence is opposed to direct evidence
(like an eyewitness who says she saw the crime occur, a confession from the defendant, or
serology evidence on semen found in a rape victim). It primarily involves coincidences or

228
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

suspicious circumstances that falsely led the police or prosecutors to believe the defendant
committed the crime. Examples include: the perpetrator told the victim he just got out of jail for
robbery and the defendant recently finished time for robbery; and the defendant was known to
have visited the victim the night of her death and threatened her.
2) Erroneous judgment on cause of death: This should also be marked under forensic error. It
involves an accidental or natural death that was erroneously ruled a homicide (such as shaken
baby syndrome cases), as well as the misdiagnosis of the method or manner of a homicide.
3) Community pressure: This should only be selected when there is a clear indication that this
was a substantial factor in the investigation or conviction of the defendant. It can be based on
either a direct statement by a criminal justice official (e.g., “we were under a lot of pressure to
solve this crime because…”) or on a post hoc interpretation by the media, scholars, or yourself.
If you are relying on the post hoc interpretation, it should be verified by the presence of special
indicators. These indicators include: the creation of a special commission to deal with the
crime(s), the horrific nature or number of crimes (e.g., a series of child rapes), a highly
publicized crime, a rushed investigation and speedy trial, and a crime involving racial tensions in
a community. Pressure from elected officials (e.g., the District Attorney or mayor) to solve a
crime can constitute community outrage
4) Other errors: Write in anything else that contributed to the erroneous indictment and/or
conviction.

w) Did the defense case present the following evidence (circle as many as apply and briefly
note the strength of the evidence presented)?

229
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Note that this should only include evidence/testimony that was initiated by the defense—if
exculpatory evidence was uncovered and used by the prosecution, then it should be recorded in
the above questions instead. Also, this question is focused on actual evidence that is
presented/used, rather than mere arguments that the defense attorney makes at trial. For example,
if an attorney in closing argument says that the eyewitnesses were mistaken and the serology
evidence is bad, this should not be included; however, if the attorney puts on a serology expert
and impeaches the eyewitness with a prior statement, then this is included).

1) Alibi evidence/witnesses (please record who the alibi witnesses were: family, friends, etc)
2) Forensic evidence (includes experts)
3) Eyewitness(es) of crime
4) Misconduct of criminal justice officials. This should be evidence of something egregious and
intentional, such as showing pictures that indicate the defendant was beaten or threatened into
confessing. Do NOT include an argument made by defense counsel that the photo array or lineup
was suggestive unless counsel produces evidence that the suggestiveness rose to the level of
misconduct (in most such cases, the defense should be able to have the lineup excluded).
5) Evidence incriminating another individual. This refers to a specific individual, e.g., arguing
that Johnny, not the defendant, committed the crime. In order to be included, the defense must
have evidence to support the claim that another individual was involved.
6) Other (describe)

230
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

58) Source(s) of error – Second conviction (Please circle as many as apply; if unknown,
write this in)
See instructions for Q 57.

59) What was the length of time:
Note that these questions measure different periods, depending on whether the case is an
erroneous conviction or a rightful acquittal/dismissal.
a) served in prison for the offense from conviction/charge to release? (write in any
correctional control imposed on defendant after release from prison)
For erroneous convictions, the time served should begin when the defendant was convicted of
the relevant crimes, even if the defendant was in prison earlier awaiting trial. It ends when the
defendant was released from prison. Please indicate whether the defendant continued to have any
serious correctional restraints placed on him after release from prison, such as parole or
registration as a sex offender (this does not include bail). If the defendant remains in prison after
the defendant’s conviction was vacated and a new trial ordered, demarcate the time spent in
prison for the convicted offense (time between conviction and vacation) from the time spent in
prison awaiting a final disposition, but include it in the total time spent in prison.
For acquittals/dismissals, the time served should begin when the defendant was charged with the
relevant crimes and end when the defendant was released from prison. If the charge date is not
known, you may use the arrest date. As above, please note whether the defendant continued to
have any serious correctional restraints placed on him after release from prison.

231
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

b) between the conviction/charge and the point at which the defendant finally exited the
criminal justice system with regards to the crime at issue?
Note that these questions measure different periods, depending on whether the case is an
erroneous conviction or a rightful acquittal/dismissal.
For erroneous convictions, the time should begin when the defendant was convicted of the
relevant crimes while for acquittals/dismissals, the time should begin when the defendant was
charged with the relevant crimes. If the charge date is not known, you may use the arrest date.
For both sets of cases, the defendant leaves the system when the government is no longer using
the criminal justice system to implicate the defendant in the relevant crime. This question is NOT
concerned with whether the defendant is still in the criminal justice system for reasons that are
not related to the crime(s) at issue (this may be indicated in Q 62), or with the time taken by the
defendant in seeking compensation or bringing a civil suit. However, if a defendant seeks a
pardon after his conviction is vacated, the grant of the pardon may be considered the date on
which the defendant exited the system.

60) Did the defendant receive compensation?
Circle “Yes” or “No.” If you do not know whether the defendant received compensation, write in
unknown. If you are aware that a claim for compensation is pending, answer “No” but write in
what the claim is, where it is pending, and any further information that will be helpful in
monitoring the progress of the claim.

232
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

61) If yes, what was the compensation for and how and when was it granted (write in)?
Indicate the source of the compensation (e.g., city police department), mechanism (e.g.,
settlement or civil judgment), and date. Also explain the reasoning behind the compensation
(e.g., awarded for a violation of the defendant’s constitutional rights, or mandatory compensation
for exonerated defendants).

62) Where is the defendant now? (write in)
Write in any relevant information about the defendant’s current life, including where he is now
located, whether he is still in prison or on parole for other offenses, and whether the defendant
has secured a job. If this information is unknown, write this in.

Coding Book Supplement: Points to Consider when Answering Question 57 (k)-(t)

Bullets for distinguishing misconduct from error:









Intentional or gross negligence (deliberate indifference)
Physical abuse in interrogations
Fabrication of evidence (as opposed to supplying information about the crime or asking
leading questions in an interrogation, which is error)
Miranda violations
Brady violations IF there is intentional or grossly negligent withholding
Fraud
Violation of ethical duties and conflicts of interest
When in doubt, benefit of the doubt goes to the state (i.e., choose error)

Bullets for distinguishing proven from alleged:




Court declares there was misconduct or error
Jury declares there was misconduct or error
Authority (police, prosecutor, governor) admits misconduct or error
233
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.




Defendant wins a jury verdict in a civil suit based on this claim
The defendant wins a large settlement (usually over $1million) on the claim AND there is
either a pattern of misconduct or error on the part of the department/office/individual or
there are additional indicia of reliability, such as an affidavit or testimony.

Bullets for distinguishing between police, prosecutor, and crime lab when responsibility
unknown:






Brady violation=prosecutor
Errors or misconduct at trial=prosecutor
Coerced confession=police
Eyewitness identification procedures=police
Losing forensic evidence or not completing tests=prosecutor, then police, then lab

Bullets for determining when an allegation should NOT be recorded:





Error stemmed from a discretionary decision
Error or misconduct was explicitly addressed and dismissed by a court (and not
overturned on appeal or subsequently discredited by another court or other officials)
Allegations of Error or misconduct has no basis in the discernible facts or is not verified
by other sources
Error or misconduct, even if true, would be trivial or harmless

The role of court decisions:




If a court dismisses a claim on the merits and is not overturned on appeal or subsequently
discredited by another court, the ruling is treated as dispositive of the merits of the claim
If a court decides there was harmless error, we code it as NO error or misconduct
A dissenting opinion is not dispositive of any point

234
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

VIII.C. Email and Call Templates for Interviews

Dear ____________,

I am contacting you as part of a federally funded social science research project to help prevent
wrongful convictions. The project, which is led by Dr. Jon Gould of American University, seeks
to understand how the criminal justice system avoids wrongful convictions by comparing felony
cases that ended in an official exoneration with those in which defendants had charges dismissed
before trial or were acquitted on the basis of their factual innocence. The project represents an
unprecedented collaboration between academic researchers and representatives of both the
prosecutorial and defense communities. As part of this project, we are working to create
thorough summaries of noted criminal cases for comparison. Eventually, we expect to examine
more than 500 cases.

We are wondering if you can help us to understand one of those cases – that of [NAME OF
DEFENDANT], which occurred in [NAME OF COUNTY AND STATE]. We have examined
several parts of this case but still have some remaining questions about the facts. Your
assistance would be a big help in completing the research and producing a report.
Attached are a summary of the research project and an outline of the several privacy protocols
we are following in the research. We want to emphasize that under federal law, nothing you tell
us will ever be released publicly, and your identity will remain confidential at all times.

235
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Accordingly, we very much hope that you will be willing to talk to us about the case and assist in
the research. Please feel free to email or call me at the contact information provided below.
With your consent, we will then ask you a set of brief questions about the [DEFENDANT’S
LAST NAME] case. If we don’t hear from you soon, we will follow up by phone in the next
week.

Thank you in advance for your time and cooperation. We look forward to your assistance.

Sincerely,

[YOUR NAME]

____________________________
[YOUR NAME AND POSITION]
Preventing Wrongful Convictions Project
American University, School of Public Affairs
4400 Massachusetts Avenue, NW
Washington, D.C. 20016
Phone: 202-885-6421
Fax: 202-885-6536
prevent@american.edu

236
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Phone Interview Template
Greeting and Introduction
Good morning, I am _____________, calling from American University’s research
project on the prevention of wrongful convictions. We communicated earlier by email and I’m
now calling to see if you might be able to assist me in collecting some missing data on the
__________case. Do you have time to answer a few questions?
[if Yes] Thanks. But before we start, I need to confirm your consent. We sent you a
document earlier outlining the nature of the project, but I need to go over some key points
briefly. Please feel free to stop me if there is anything you don’t understand or that concerns
you. At the end, I’ll ask for your verbal consent.
Informed Consent
o This research is being conducted by researchers from American University and is funded
by the National Institute of Justice, U.S. Department of Justice.
o The interview will ask you about your knowledge of or experience with a particular
criminal case that has since concluded.
o There are no benefits to you as a participant other than to further research about the
sources of wrongful convictions.
o The foreseeable risks of participating in this project may include unpleasant memories of
the case. Should this occur and the effects be serious, researchers will assist in referring
you to a counselor.
o The researchers will be the only individuals with knowledge of your participation and
access to your responses. Any notes or recordings taken during the interviews will be
immediately secured and then destroyed at the conclusion of the study. Nothing released
from this study will state or imply that you have spoken with the research team. Federal
protection prevents the release of any information obtained from this research, even by
subpoena.
o Your participation is voluntary, and you may withdraw from the study at any time and for
any reason. If you decide not to participate, or if you withdraw from the study, there is
no penalty; nor are there any costs to you or any other party for participating in the
research.
o This research has been reviewed according to American University procedures. You may
contact the University Office of Research Subject Protections if you have questions or
comments regarding your rights as a participant in the research.
o Based on what I have just told you, do you agree to participate in this study?

237
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

o [If Yes] Thanks. I will send you a hard copy of the full consent form after we finish the
interview. That copy is for you to review and keep.

Interview
Continue the phone interview with specific questions relating to the case(s). If the interviewee
has questions or concerns about the project that were not already addressed, see below for
scripted answers. If the interviewee is still not comfortable with the answers or has a concern
you cannot address, ask them if a project supervisor may contact them to answer their questions.
If so, ask when would be a good time for someone to get back to them and then terminate the
interview.


Project Methodology and Goals: Can you explain the project a little? What are you doing
with this data?
o The purpose of the project is to compare wrongful convictions and innocencebased dismissals/acquittals in order to identify which factors explain how the
criminal justice system identifies and addresses cases of innocence. We are
looking at cases involving charges for violent felonies since 1980, in which the
defendant was later found to be factually innocent.
o Once we have about 250 cases each of wrongful convictions and innocence-based
dismissals and acquittals, we will run several statistical tests using matched
comparison samples to identify which factors help to explain the different
outcomes of these cases. We will also have an expert panel look over the
individual case files to assess how and why the cases ended differently.
o In the end, we will develop a database of cases and a report outlining our analysis
that will be archived at the National Institute of Justice.



Project Visibility: Who is going to see this? How will the data be presented?
o The National Institute of Justice will release the final report of this project. All
sensitive information such as names and geographical area will be removed
before publication.



Project Participants: Who is involved?
o The Principal Investigator of the project is Dr. Jon Gould, a professor American
University.
o The project is funded by the National Institute of Justice, the research and
evaluation arm of the Dep’t of Justice.

238
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

o Other collaborators include the National District Attorneys Association, the Police
Foundation, forensic specialists, representatives of the defense bar, and the
Innocence Project.


Question of Innocence: How do you know these people are innocent?
o We are using a very narrow definition of factual innocence, a more exacting
standard than legal innocence. In our project, factual innocence means that there
must be facts to reasonably conclude the defendant did not commit the crime. But
in addition, we also require an official recognition of this innocence, for instance
from a prosecutor, judge, or jury.
o [you should be prepared to explain briefly why the case you are asking about fits
our definition of innocence]



General Hostility: Why should I help the project?
o We understand your concerns, but the goal of this project is not to point fingers at
any particular part of the criminal justice system but rather to help the system
learn from its successes in identifying innocent defendants and preventing
wrongful convictions. We need your assistance in creating unbiased, complete
case files.



Follow-up Contact: I can’t talk now or I need to look up some information and get back
to you. Whom should I contact? Where should I send documents?
o It is up to the interviewee how they would like to provide us with the information.
Over the phone or via fax is most secure.
o Phone: (202) 885-6421
o Fax: (202) 885-6536 Attn: Preventing Wrongful Convictions Project
o Email: prevent@american.edu
o Mailing address: Preventing Wrongful Convictions Project
School of Public Affairs
4400 Massachusetts Ave., NW
Washington, DC 20016-8022

Closing
I think those are all the questions I have for now. Is there anyone else you would advise me to
talk to about this case?
[If Yes, take down relevant contact information]

239
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Thank you for your assistance; you’ve been very helpful. Again, my name is _____________
and please feel free to contact me if any questions or concerns come up.

240
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Attachment #1 for Interviewees
Project Overview: How the Criminal Justice System Prevents Wrongful Convictions
A Study by American University with Funding from the National Institute of Justice
What is the purpose of the study?
The goal of the research is to ascertain which factors explain how the criminal justice
system identifies and addresses cases of factual innocence to prevent wrongful convictions. To
accomplish this, the project is comparing felony cases that ended in an official exoneration with
those in which defendants had charges dismissed or were acquitted on the basis of their factual
innocence.

What is involved?
To be included in the study, cases must involve indictments for violent felonies post-1980 in
which the defendant was later found to be factually innocent. Such cases are divided into two
sets. One group involves official exonerations based on factual innocence. The other includes
post-indictment dismissals or acquittals of defendants who were factually innocent. Researchers
are identifying qualifying cases and then coding each for more than 50 factors.
The team will run several statistical tests using matched comparison samples to identify
which factors help to explain the different outcomes of these cases. Researchers will augment
these results by convening an expert panel of prosecutors, defense attorneys, retired judges,
police detectives, and forensic scientists, among others, to assess independently how and why the
cases ended differently.

What constitutes “innocence”?
This project uses a narrow definition of factual innocence. To qualify under this study,
there must be clear evidence that defendants did not commit the crime as well as an official

241
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

declaration – whether by a prosecutor, judge, jury, compensation board, or governor – that the
defendant was factually innocent.

Who is conducting the research?
The project is led by Jon Gould, professor at American University. Professor Gould
previously has directed studies for the National Institute of Justice and the federal courts, among
others. He is aided by Professor Richard Leo of the University of San Francisco. Dr. Gould and
Dr. Leo are both lawyers and social scientists. Together, they direct a research team of four
others.

Who is funding the study?
Research is supported by the National Institute of Justice, the research arm of the U.S.
Department of Justice.

What other groups are involved?
This project represents a unique collaboration between academic researchers, the
National District Attorneys Association, the Police Foundation, defense advocates, former
judges, forensic scientists, and representatives of the Innocence Project. Together, these diverse
participants will ensure that the research is valid and reliable.

What are participants asked to do?
Individuals are asked to assist the research team in collecting data on cases that qualify
under the study’s criteria. Researchers will contact willing participants with brief questions

242
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

about specific cases that the individual may have information about. As described below, no
information provided will ever be disclosed.

What safeguards are in place?
In obtaining funding from the National Institute of Justice, the researchers have secured a
privacy certificate from the Department of Justice. Any data collected or information secured as
part of this project is protected from disclosure or release, even by subpoena. The research team
is also bound by human subjects protections from American University. Please see the
accompanying Summary of Human Subjects Research Protections for more detail.

243
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Attachment #2 for Interviewees

Summary of Human Subjects Research Protections

The research team is taking several measures to protect the confidentiality and integrity
of the information acquired and to minimize discomfort to participants in the research.
Training and Confidentiality Agreements
All members of the research team have completed human subjects research training
required by American University. In addition, all researchers, expert panelists, and consultants
will sign confidentiality agreements.
Documents and Data Received from Participants
No confidential information or documents received from participants, including notes
from interviews, will ever be released publicly. All hard-copied documents will be kept in a
locked filing cabinet in a locked office. Likewise, electronic databases or spreadsheets will be
maintained in a password-protected format and on password-protected computers maintained in
the locked offices of the principal investigator and the coding supervisor.
Project Database and Case Narratives
The project will create a statistical database that codes cases for the presence of various
possible facts as well as case narratives for select cases. Cases will be assigned a code number,
rather than the defendant’s name, in the database. Likewise, researchers will use monikers in
constructing the narratives so as not to identity any person involved in the case. Those
narratives, which will be used only by the research team, will omit the geographic location of
any case, instead simply stating whether a case occurred in an urban, rural, or suburban location.
When the project is completed, only the principal investigator will keep a copy of the
database, referenced list of case names, and case narratives, which he will maintain in a locked
cabinet in a locked office. A version of the database will be made available for the National
Institute of Justice to archive, but the archived version will be “scrubbed” of any information that
would allow an analyst to speculate and thus identify any particular case.

244
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

VIII.D. Informed Consent Form for Interviews

Social Science Research on Wrongful Convictions Informed Consent Form
RESEARCH PROCEDURES
This research is being conducted by researchers from American University to study the sources
of wrongful convictions. It is funded by the National Institute of Justice, U.S. Department of
Justice (NIJ).
This research is being conducted to understand how cases of wrongful conviction differ from
those in which an innocent suspect is cleared or acquitted before a conviction occurs. If you
agree to participate, you will be asked to take part in an interview of 30-90 minutes, whether in
person or by phone, at your convenience. The interview will ask you about your knowledge of
or experience with a particular criminal case that has since concluded, whether a wrongful
conviction that was followed by exoneration or an arrest and initial prosecution of a factuallyinnocent suspect who was cleared or acquitted before a conviction at trial. NIJ personnel may be
present during the interview, although you may ask for them to be excluded.
RISKS
The foreseeable risks or discomforts of participating in this project may include unpleasant
memories of the case. Should this occur and should the effects be serious, researchers will assist
in referring you to a counselor or a victim assistance coordinator, whichever is most appropriate.
BENEFITS
There are no benefits to you as a participant other than to further research about the sources of
wrongful convictions.
CONFIDENTIALITY
The researchers will be the only individuals with knowledge of your participation and access to
your responses. They will keep your identity and comments confidential. Interviewers may
create summaries of case facts for discussions within the research team, but they will employ
monikers. Any notes or recordings taken during the interviews will be kept confidential by the
researchers and will be destroyed at the conclusion of the study. Nothing released from this
study will state or imply that you have spoken with the research team, and the researchers will
not publish any information you provide that would identify you. Federal protection prevents the
release of any information obtained from this research, even by subpoena.

245
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

PARTICIPATION
Your participation is voluntary, and you may withdraw from the study at any time and for any
reason. If you decide not to participate, or if you withdraw from the study, there is no penalty;
nor are there any costs to you or any other party for participating in the research. You should
feel free to ask any questions about this project or your participation with the researchers at any
time.
CONTACT
This research is led by Professor Jon Gould of the School of Public Affairs at American
University. Professor Gould may be reached at 202-885-6535 or gould@american.edu for
questions or to report a research-related problem. You may contact the American University
Institutional Review Board at 202-885-3447 if you have questions or comments regarding your
rights as a participant in the research.
This research has been reviewed according to American University procedures governing your
participation in this research.
CONSENT
I have read this form and agree to participate in this study.
__________________________
Name (print)

__________________________
Signature

__________________________
Date

__________________________
Name of Witness/Interviewer

__________________________
Signature of Witness/Interviewer

__________________________
Date
246
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

247
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

VIII.E. Statistical Coding Book

*Use CAPS for “write in” variables
**Assign “99” to any variable if it is non-applicable
***When making estimations of days, a month consists of 31 days

BACKGROUND AND JURISDICTIONAL INFORMATION

Defendant’s name (def): string

[Question #1] This is the full legal name used on court documents. Last name, first name and
middle name or initial.

Identification code (id): string

This will be assigned after the cases are coded. It consists of C or A (conviction or acquittal) and
a unique number.

Innocence Project Case (IP_case): nominal
0=no
1=yes

248
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #2] Select “Yes” if there is an Innocence Project case profile for the case.

Type of case (case_type): nominal
0=near miss
1=erroneous conviction

State where conviction/charge occurred (state): string

[Question #25] Write in the state where conviction occurred. Use U.S. Post Office abbreviations.

Numerical state where conviction/charge occurred (state_num): nominal

This is a variable you will not need to enter manually. The syntax will assign a variable based on
the state you inputted. Recode of (state) variable.

County where conviction/charge occurred (county): string

[Question #25] Write in the county or parish where conviction occurred. Do not use
abbreviations.

Region (region): nominal
0=New England (CT, ME, MA, NH, RI, VT)

249
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=Middle Atlantic (NJ, NY, PA)
2=East North Central (IN, IL, MI, OH, WI)
3=West North Central (IA, KS, MN, MO, NE, ND, SD)
4=South Atlantic (DE, DC, FL, GA, MD, NC, SC, VA, WV)
5=East South Central (AL, KY, MS, TN)
6=West South Central (AR, LA, OK, TX)
7=Mountain (AZ, CO, ID, NM, MT, UT, NV, WY)
8=Pacific (AK, CA, HI, OR, WA)

This is a variable you will not need to enter manually. The syntax will assign a variable based on
the state you inputted. Regions and districts are defined by the US Census.

Former Confederate State (confed_state): nominal
0=no
1=yes

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter. The syntax will assign a variable based on
the state you input. Confederate states are defined as states that seceded from the Union—South
Carolina, Mississippi, Florida, Alabama, Georgia, Louisiana, Texas, Virginia, Arkansas,
Tennessee, and North Carolina.

State Death Penalty Culture (dp_cult): scale

250
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter. It is based upon the per capita executions
since Furman (1976) and will be assigned according to the state you input.

State Death Penalty Culture (dp_cult2): scale

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter. It is calculated as the number of
executions since Furman (1976) per number of murders and will be assigned according to the
state you input

Voting and political ideology (pol_ideol): nominal

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter. It is based upon the median vote for
president (party) from 1980 to 2008 and will be assigned according to the state you input.

Judicial selection (jud_sel): nominal
0=partisan election
1=non-partisan election
2=gubernatorial appointment
3=legislative appointment

This variable is based on the method of judicial selection for the criminal trial court in the
county in which the crime occurred or was prosecuted.

251
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

State crime rate 5 years prior (crime_rate_5yrs): scale

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter. It is the median crime rate for 5 years
prior to the crime and will be assigned according to the state you input.

State crime rate year of the crime (crime_rate): scale

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter. It is the crime rate at the time the crime
occurred and will be assigned according to the state you input.

Crime consistency (crime_consistency): scale

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter. It is the percent change in crime rate from
median of 5 years prior to the crime and the crime rate at the time the crime occurred. It will be
assigned according to the state you input.

DEMOGRAPHIC INFORMATION

High school graduate at time of offense (hs_grad): nominal
0=no
1=yes

252
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #5] This refers to level of education obtained prior to arrest for the crime at issue
here. This includes a GED.

Defendant has cognitive impairment or intellectual disability (MR): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #6] This is at the time of the offense. It is an objective standard, so do not consider
whether the impairment or disability was known at the time the defendant was arrested and/or
brought to trial. However, only impairments or disabilities that existed at the time of the crime or
the defendant’s indictment should be noted (i.e., disabilities developed after conviction,
dismissal, or acquittal are irrelevant). This question is meant to be as inclusive as possible and
includes emotional and personality disorders as well as low intelligence. Generally, if a
defendant has an IQ below 80 (WAIS-IV), mark “yes”. If there is no indication whatsoever that
the defendant had a mental condition, you may assume “no”.

Fluent in English (English): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #7] Choose “No” if there is any indication that defendant was not conversant or
could not write in English. If there is no reasonable expectation that the defendant spoke another
language, choose “Yes”.

253
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Previous Criminal Convictions (prior_crim_hx): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #3] This includes any convictions in any jurisdiction in the United States before the
time of arrest for the crime in question here. Do not count simple driving citations.
[Note, Question #2 asks you to record if the defendant has previously been arrested or charged,
but not convicted, for any crime.]

Number of prior criminal convictions (prior_convict): scale

[Question #4] Be as precise as possible, but if you can estimate, do so. If the actual number is
not known but you have an idea of how many convictions there were (i.e., only a few, several,
many), use the following answers: 96=a few, 3 or less; 97=more than 3.

Criminal history similar to instant offense (hx_sim): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no criminal history)

This will require some interpretation by the coder, but the following are offered as guidelines:
sexual assault crimes should be designated as similar to other sexual assaults (despite different
MOs); serious assaults with a deadly weapon should be designated as similar to murder or

254
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

attempted murder; property crimes should be designated as similar to other property crimes
unless the difference in severity is enormous. In addition, a previous crime against the same
person can be considered similar.
One similar criminal conviction is sufficient to answer “Yes”.

History of gang affiliation (gang): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #8] This refers to the defendant only. If no mention is made of possible gang
affiliation, select “No”. By itself, participating in the organized sale of narcotics is NOT enough
to indicate gang affiliation.

Number of Codefendants at trial (num_codef): scale

[Question #9] Co-defendant is used in the legal sense, as an individual indicted for the same
crime and not subsequently severed at trial. An individual is still considered a co-defendant if
(s)he did not go to trial with the defendant because one or the other disposed of his/her case
through a plea.

Number of Additional Alleged Co-Perpetrators (num_perp): scale

255
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #10] This is the number and name of any perpetrators other than the defendant but
including co-defendants, who the police or prosecution alleges participated in the crime(s) for
which the defendant was convicted. [Question #7 also asks that you briefly indicate whether the
co-perpetrators were ever arrested and charged, and if so, what was the disposition of their
case]

Co-perpetrator confessed (coperp_confessed): nominal
0=co-perpetrator did not confess
1=co-perpetrator confessed
99=not applicable

Select “co-perpetrator confessed” if the co-perpetrator made incriminating statements (a full
confession is not necessary), regardless of whether the confession was later proved false. This
only refers to confessions or statements that occurred before or during the defendant’s
conviction or case. If the identity of the perpetrator is unknown or he/she was never
apprehended, enter ‘99’.

Co-perpetrator implicated current defendant (coperp_imp): nominal
0=co-perpetrator did not implicated defendant
1=co-perpetrator implicated defendant
99=not applicable

256
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Select “co-perpetrator implicated defendant” if the co-perpetrator’s statements implicated the
defendant’s participation in the crime, at any time in the investigation and/or trial. If the identity
of the perpetrator is unknown or he/she was never apprehended, enter ‘99’.

Co-perpetrator guilty (coperp_guilty): nominal
0=co-perpetrator not guilty
1=co-perpetrator guilty
99=not applicable

[Question # 11] Answer “Yes” if the co-defendant or co-perpetrator was convicted (or pled
guilty) and was never exonerated. Answer “No” if a co-defendant or co-perpetrator was found
guilty and later exonerated. If the identity of the perpetrator is unknown or he/she was never
apprehended, enter ‘99’.

Number of Victims in Crime Charged (num_vic_charged): scale

[Question #12] This is the number of victims involved in crime(s) for which the defendant was
charged together in the relevant jurisdiction.

Number of Victims in Crime Convicted (num_vic_conv): scale

[Question #12] This is the number of victims involved in crime(s) of which the defendant was
convicted together in the relevant jurisdiction.

257
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

*Note, while we make the distinction here between victims of crimes for which the defendant was
convicted and victims of crimes for which the defendant was merely charged, all victims
regardless remain designated as “victims” in the rest of the coding (e.g., if there are 3 rape
victims at trial, but the defendant is only convicted of one of the rapes, the other 2 victims are
still treated as victims for the questions regarding age, race, eyewitness account, etc,).

Race/ethnicity of Defendant (def_race): nominal
0=Caucasian/white
1=African American/black
2=Hispanic/Latino
3=Native American
4=Asian
5=Other

[Question #13] Use the race/ethnicity designated either by official record or by colloquial usage
(with the official record taking precedent if there is a discrepancy). You may also choose a race
or ethnicity based upon a photograph; assumptions may be made from clearly ethnic names, but
this should only be done as a last resort and where the name is unambiguous . ‘Other’ includes
individuals of Asian Indian, Middle Eastern, or mixed descent.

Race/ethnicity of First Victim (vic_race1): nominal
0=Caucasian/white

258
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=African American/black
2=Hispanic/Latino
3=Native American
4=Asian
5=Other

[Question #14] See instructions for race/ethnicity of defendant.

Race/ethnicity of victim one and defendant (race_diff1): nominal
0=same
1=different

If the race/ethnicity of the defendant AND victim are designated as ‘other’ but you know that the
races are actually different, select ‘1’.

Race/ethnicity of Second Victim (vic_race2): nominal
0=Caucasian/white
1=African American/black
2=Hispanic/Latino
3=Native American
4=Asian
5=Other
99=not applicable

259
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #14] See instructions above.

Race/ethnicity of victim two and defendant (race_diff2): nominal
0=same
1=different
99=not applicable

Continue for victims #3-10

Relationship Between Victim 1 and Offender (relationship1): ordinal or nominal
0=stranger
1=proximity but no known relationship (i.e., neighbor, worked in vicinity/same
location, attended same school)
2=friend, acquaintance
3=family or significant other

[Question #15] This is a scaled variable. Therefore, select the closest relationship the victim
and defendant ever had (i.e., if the victim was a former girlfriend, this is still “3”, or if a friend
is also a neighbor, still select ‘2’. “Neighbor” includes anyone whose physical living proximity
to the victim is noted in official reports or the media (i.e., said to have been living a few streets
down, in an adjacent apartment complex, etc.). Family member/significant other includes
extended family by birth or marriage, as well as any close romantic relationship.

260
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Relationship Between Victim 2 and Offender (relationship2): ordinal or nominal
0=stranger
1=proximity but no known relationship (i.e., neighbor, worked in vicinity/same
location, attended same school)
2=friend, acquaintance
3=family, significant other
99=not applicable

[Question #15] See instructions above.

Continue for victims #3-10

Age of Defendant (age): scale

[Question #16] This refers to the age of the defendant at the time of the offense. If there is a
range, use the mean age. If you really only have a rough estimate, use the following system: if it
says defendant was in his 20s, use ‘25’; if it says he was in his early 20s, use ‘21’; if it says late
20s, use ‘29’; if it says late 20s or early 30s, use ‘30’…and so on.

Age of First Victim (vic_age1): scale

261
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #17] This refers to the age of the victim(s) at the time of the offense. See above for
estimates.

Age of Second Victim (vic_age2): scale

[Question #17] See instructions above.

Continue for victims #3-10

Gender of Defendant (gender): nominal
0=male
1=female

[Question #18]

Gender of the First Victim (vic_gender1): nominal
0=male
1=female

[Question #19]

Gender of the Second Victim (vic_gender2): nominal
0=male

262
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=female
99=not applicable

[Question #19]

Continue for victims #3-10

At least one female victim (Addfemale):
0=no
1=yes

You will not need to enter this manually. Syntax will calculate whether at least one victim was a
female.

At least one white female victim (Addwhitefemale):
0=no
1=yes

You will not need to enter this manually. Syntax will calculate whether at least one victim was a
female.

263
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

CHARGING AND INVESTIGATIVE INFORMATION

Charged with robbery (c_rob): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20] This includes attempted robbery.

Charged with attempted kidnapping (c_att_kidnap): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20]

Charged with kidnapping/false imprisonment (c_kidnap): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20]

Charged with attempted rape (c_sexual_assault): nominal
0=no
1=yes

264
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #20] This includes any felony that involves sexual misconduct.

Charged with rape/sexual assault (c_sexual_assault): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20] This includes any felony that involves sexual misconduct.

Charged with assault/battery (c_assault): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20]

Charged with burglary (c_burglary): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20] This includes attempted burglary and home invasion.

265
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Charged with child abuse-incl sexual assault (c_child_abuse): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20] This refers to a charge that specifically includes as part of the offense that it was
committed against a child or minor. Thus, do not select this option if a defendant was charged
with raping an 8 yr old girl, if the charge is simply “rape”. However, DO select this option if the
offense was “Murder of a child”.

Charged with carjacking (c_carjacking): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20]

Charged with attempted murder (c_att_murder): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20]

266
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Charged with murder (c_murder): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20]

Charged with possession of a weapon (c_weapon): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20] This includes aggravated offenses that stem from the possession of a weapon or
offenses that include the possession or use of a weapon (i.e., armed violence).

Charged with arson (c_arson): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #20]

Total crimes charged with (charges): string

List of all charges.

267
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Number of charges (num_charges): scale

You will need to enter this manually; include every count as a separate charge.

Additive Charges (AddCharge): nominal

You will not need to enter this manually: “Do If” loops
c_murder and c_sexual_assault=1
c_murder and NOT c_sexual_assault=2
c_sexual_assault and NOT c_murder=3

Convicted of robbery (rob): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21] This includes attempted robbery.

Convicted of attempted kidnapping (att_kidnap): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21]

268
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Convicted of kidnapping/false imprisonment (kidnap): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21]

Convicted of attempted rape (att_sexual_assault): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21] This includes any felony that involves sexual misconduct.

Convicted of rape/sexual assault (sexual_assault): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21] This includes any felony that involves sexual misconduct.

Convicted of assault/battery (assault): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21]

269
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Convicted of burglary (burglary): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21] This includes attempted burglary and home invasion.

Convicted of child abuse-incl sexual assault (child_abuse): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21] This refers to a conviction that specifically includes as part of the offense that it
was committed against a child or minor. Thus, do not select this option if a defendant was
convicted of raping an 8 yr old girl, if the conviction is simply “rape”. However, DO select this
option if the offense was “Murder of a child”.

Convicted of carjacking (carjacking): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21]

Convicted of attempted murder (att_murder): nominal

270
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

0=no
1=yes

[Question #21]

Convicted of murder (murder): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21]

Convicted of possession of a weapon (weapon): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21] This includes aggravated offenses that stem from the possession of a weapon or
offenses that include the possession or use of a weapon (i.e., armed violence).

Convicted of arson (arson): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #21]

271
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Total number of convictions in erroneous conviction case (num_convictions): scale

You will need to enter this manually; include every count as a separate conviction. An
aggravated offense is considered one conviction, even if you selected “weapons” as a separate
variable, above.

[Question #21]

Total convictions in erroneous conviction case (convictions): string

[Question #21] Write in the convictions.

Additive Convictions (AddConv): nominal

You will not need to enter this manually: “Do If” loops
murder and sexual_assault=1
murder and NOT sexual_assault=2
sexual_assault and NOT murder=3

Most serious offense convicted of (serious_offense): ordinal/scale
0=no conviction
1=Aggravated assault

272
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

2=Burglary
3=Robbery
4=Arson
5=Kidnapping
6=Attempted rape
7=Attempted murder
8=Rape
9=Murder

Choose the highest number that corresponds to a crime of which the defendant was convicted.
The list is based on federal crime seriousness categories used in sentencing.

Crime part of serial crime event (serial): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #22] This was not directly asked on the original narrative coding sheet, so you may
have to imply the information from what is provided or do more research to obtain an answer.
Answer “Yes” if police or prosecutors thought then or think now that the crime at issue was part
of a serial crime event. A serial murder is defined as: “the unlawful killing of two or more
victims by the same offender(s), in separate events.” [FBI Serial Murder Report] A serial rape is
similarly defined as: “the unlawful sexual assault of two or more victims by the same offender(s),
in separate events.”

273
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Date of crime (date_crime): mm/dd/yyyy

[Question #23] If the day is unknown, use the first of the month. If there are more than one
crimes charged together, there is an individual variable for each crime. Please put them in
chronological order if possible. If there is the possibility of two dates, i.e., perhaps the murder
occurred at 11pm or perhaps at 5 am the next day, use the earlier date. [Question #19 asks you
to write in the date if known; if the day is unknown, write this in]

Date of initial arrest (date_arrest): mm/dd/yyyy

[Question #24] If the day is unknown, use the first of the month. As above, use the earlier date if
there is a question between two possible dates. [Question #20 asks you to write in the date if
known; if the day is unknown, write this in]. If defendant is arrested and released, and then
arrested again, use the first arrest date.

Date of charge/indictment (date_indictment): mm/dd/yyyy

[Question #24] This refers to the date that the information is filed by the prosecutor, or in states
with grand juries, when the jury hands down the indictment (rather than the day the police
charge the defendant). If the day is unknown, use the first of the month.
If the charge/indictment date is not available, you may use the arraignment date.
If the charge date is different from the indictment date, use the indictment date.

274
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #24 asks you to write in the date if known; if the day is unknown, write this in]

Date of trial court resolution (date_resolution): mm/dd/yyyy

[Question 30] For erroneous convictions, use the date of the first conviction (rather than
subsequent convictions or a hung jury or mistrial). Do not use the sentencing date, but if
possible, you may make an estimate of the conviction date based upon the sentencing date. If the
day is unknown, use the first of the month. Again, if there is the possibility of one or more
consecutive dates, choose the earliest.

[Question #30 asks you to write in the date if known; if the day is unknown, write this in]

Resolution post-DNA period (PostDNAresoltuion): nominal
0=no
1=yes

You will not need to enter this manually. Syntax will assign a value based on whether the
resolution you entered was on or after 1/1/1989.

Length of time between crime and arrest (time_arrest): scale

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter.

275
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Length of time between crime and charge/indictment (time_indictment): scale

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter.

Length of time between crime and trial or trial resolution (time_trial): scale

This is a variable you will not need to manually enter.

County Sheriff investigates case (sheriff): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #26] This should be the agency that took responsibility for bringing the defendant to
prosecution. If the agency had only a minor, brief role in the case, such as arresting the
defendant and handing him over to another agency, or providing information about a suspect, do
not include it. However, you may select more than one agency. Answer “Yes” for any law
enforcement agency at the county level (even if it is not called the Sheriff’s Dept).

Local Police investigates case (local_police): nominal
0=no
1=yes

276
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #26] This should be the agency that took responsibility for bringing the defendant to
prosecution. If the agency had only a minor, brief role in the case, such as arresting the
defendant and handing him over to another agency, or providing information about a suspect, do
not include it. However, you may select more than one agency.

State Police investigates case (state_police): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #26] This should be the agency that took responsibility for bringing the defendant to
prosecution. If the agency had only a minor, brief role in the case, such as arresting the
defendant and handing him over to another agency, or providing information about a suspect, do
not include it. However, you may select more than one agency.

State bureau of investigation investigates case (SBI): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #26] This should be the agency that took responsibility for bringing the defendant to
prosecution. If the agency had only a minor, brief role in the case, such as arresting the
defendant and handing him over to another agency, or providing information about a suspect, do
not include it. However, you may select more than one agency.

277
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Federal law enforcement investigates case (Fed_LE): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #26] This should be the agency that took responsibility for bringing the defendant to
prosecution. If the agency had only a minor, brief role in the case, such as arresting the
defendant and handing him over to another agency, or providing information about a suspect, do
not include it. However, you may select more than one agency.

CASE PROCEDURE

Multiple trials to reach disposition (multiple_trials): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #40] Indicate “Yes” if initial trial(s) resulted in no verdict (i.e., a hung jury or a
mistrial) and a subsequent trial led to a verdict. Select “No” if there was only one trial or if
there were multiple trials with a guilty verdict each time. Select “Not applicable” if it is a
dismissal case.

Method of Case Disposition at First Disposition (disposition1): nominal

278
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

0=dismissal before trial
1=jury trial
2=bench trial
3=plea bargain
4=defense motion
5=prosecution motion
6=joint motion
7=sua sponte
8=other

[Question #32] If there are multiple methods of case disposition, select “other”.

Sentence Received (sentence): nominal
0=less than 10 years
1=10 years to 24 years
2=25 years to life
3=life sentence (including life plus)
4=LWOP
5=death
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #33] If the defendant received multiple sentences that were to be served concurrently,
you should select the highest sentence that the defendant received. If he received sentences to be

279
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

served consecutively, add the sentences together. If the sentence was a range other than “x”years to life, take the average and use that number. If the sentence was several times the normal
life expectancy of an individual (e.g. 3,000 years), select “3”.

Conviction Appealed (appeal): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable, no conviction

[Question #34]

Highest level direct appeal heard (app_level): nominal
0=no appeal
1=trial court
2=state appellate court
3=state supreme court/highest state court
4=United States Supreme Court
99=not applicable, no conviction

[Questions #35 and #37] This means the highest level of court that agreed to hear arguments on
the appeal—not petitions for hearings that were denied. Please note that in Texas, the highest
state court of appeals is the Texas Court of Criminal Appeals. The same with Oklahoma.

280
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Final outcome of direct appeals process (app_outcome): nominal
0=no appeal
1=conviction affirmed
2=conviction overturned and thrown out
3=conviction overturned and new trial
4=other/mixed with at least one affirmation
99=not applicable, no conviction

[Questions #36-37] This refers to the final outcome of the direct appeals process for the first
conviction only. You will have to look first at Question #36 to determine the result of the initial
appeal and then look at Question #37 to determine if there was a further appeal and if so, what
was the result. We are only concerned with the court’s disposition of the substantive
conviction(s) at issue. Thus, if the court affirms the conviction but remands for re-sentencing,
this will be counted as an affirmation. You may assume “conviction affirmed” for an erroneous
conviction if there is no mention of a further appeal and it is apparent that the conviction is not
overturned until a long time after the initial appeal. “Other/mixed” should be marked when the
court overturns one or more of the convictions, but also affirms one or more of the convictions
(thus leaving the defendant in prison with at least one erroneous conviction).

Multiple guilty verdicts, i.e. guilty verdict(s) upon retrial (multiple_guilt): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

281
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[See Question #42]

Method of disposition at re-trial post appeal (later_disposition): nominal
0=dismissal before trial
1=jury trial
2=bench trial
3=plea bargain
4=defense motion
5=prosecution motion
6=joint motion by prosecution and defense
7=sua sponte
8=jury acquittal
9=bench acquittal
10=other
99=not applicable

[Question #43]

Sentence Received at retrial (second_sentence): nominal
0=less than 10 years
1=10 years to 24 years
2=25 years to life

282
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

3=life sentence (including life plus)
5=LWOP
6=death
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #44]

Second Conviction Appealed (second_appeal): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #45]

Final outcome of second appeals process (sec_app_outcome): nominal
1=conviction affirmed
2=conviction overturned and thrown out
3=conviction overturned and new trial
4=other/mixed with at least one affirmation
99=not applicable, no appeal

[Question #47-48] This refers to the final outcome of the direct appeals process for the second
conviction only. You will have to look first at Question #47 to determine the result of the initial

283
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

appeal and then look at Question #48 to determine if there was a further appeal and if so, what
was the result. We are only concerned with the court’s disposition of the substantive
conviction(s) at issue. Thus, if the court affirms the conviction but remands for re-sentencing,
this will be counted as an affirmation. You may assume “conviction affirmed” for an erroneous
conviction if there is no mention of a further appeal and it is apparent that the conviction is not
overturned until a long time after the initial appeal. “Other/mixed” should be marked when the
court overturns one or more of the convictions, but also affirms one or more of the convictions
(thus leaving the defendant in prison with at least one erroneous conviction).

Same District Attorney in office at conviction and exoneration (same_DA): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

Type of defense attorney at original proceedings (trial_atty): nominal
0=public defender/court appointed
1=pro bono
2=private/retained
3=pro se
4=mixed/other

[Question #51]

284
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Type of defense attorney at later proceedings (later_atty): nominal
0=public defender/court appointed
1=pro bono
2=private/retained
3=pro se
4=public defender, pro bono
5=public defender, private
6=public defender, pro se
7=public defender, pro se, pro bono
8=public defender, private, pro bono
9=private, pro bono

As in the narrative coding sheet, do not include attorneys who represented the defendant for a
purely civil claim, such as compensation.

Factual innocence based on no crime occurring (no_crime): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #52]

Factual innocence based on physical impossibility (phys_imp): nominal

285
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

0=no
1=yes

[Question #52]

Factual innocence based on DNA (exon_DNA): nominal
0=No
1=Yes

[Question #52]

Factual innocence based on scientific evidence other than DNA (scientific_evid): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #52]

Factual innocence based on identification of the true perpetrator (true_perp_id): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #52] This includes any identification of the true perpetrator by the state, including
police, prosecutor, or crime lab (it is not essential that they are arrested, charged, or convicted).

286
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Do not include it if there is no real verification of the real perpetrator's identify (if, for example,
the defendant just alleges that Joe committed the crime).

Factual innocence based on recantation of witness (exon_recant): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #52] This includes the recantation of crucial prosecution witnesses, which the state
credits.

Factual innocence based on other evidence/information (exon_other): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #52]

Factual innocence based on DNA and identification of the true perpetrator (DNAtrueperp):
nominal

You will not need to enter this manually: “Do If” loop
exon_dna and true_perp_id=1

Total indicators of factual innocence (total_innocence): scale

287
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

You will not need to enter this manually. It is a count variable that adds up all the ‘1’s on the
previous “factual innocence based on…” variables.

Exoneration method (exon_method): nominal
0=Governor’s pardon
1=State trial court overturned conviction with prejudice
2=State appellate court overturned conviction with prejudice
3=Highest state court overturned conviction with prejudice
4=State court of unknown level overturned conviction with prejudice
5=Federal district court overturned conviction with prejudice
6=Federal circuit court overturned conviction with prejudice
7=U.S. Supreme Court overturned conviction with prejudice
8=State trial court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered,
prosecution dismissed
9=State appellate court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered,
prosecution dismissed
10=Highest state court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered,
prosecution dismissed
11= State court of unknown level overturned conviction with prejudice
12=Federal district court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered,
prosecution dismissed

288
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

13=Federal circuit court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered,
prosecution dismissed
14=U.S. Supreme Court overturned conviction without prejudice, new trial ordered,
prosecution dismissed
15=Court overturned conviction, new trial was ordered, and acquitted
16=Other
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #53] If you know that the conviction was overturned by a court and the prosecutor
chose not to refile charges, but you do not know the type of court, choose 11 if there is absolutely
no indication a federal court was involved.

Received a pardon by the governor (pardoned): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #54] Mark an answer, regardless of whether a pardon was the source of exoneration.

Date of exoneration (date_exon): mm/dd/yyyy.

[Question #56] You must write in the first date on which an exoneration occurred. If the
exoneration occurred through a governor’s pardon, use the date of the pardon. If the
exoneration occurred through legislative action, use the date the bill was passed. If the

289
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

exoneration occurred through a court declaration of innocence, or an overturned or set aside
conviction with prejudice, use the date of the court’s opinion or order. If the exoneration
occurred through other court action, coupled with a prosecutor’s decision to dismiss charges or
vacate the conviction, use the date at which this action was given legal effect (i.e. the date that
the court grants the motion to vacate or the date that the prosecutor dismisses the charges).

Length of time between trial resolution and exoneration (time_to_exoner): scale

This will be calculated for you.

Length of time served from conviction to release (time_served): scale

[Question #59(a)] Write in. Measured in months. For erroneous convictions, the time served
should begin when the defendant was convicted of the relevant crimes, even if the defendant was
in prison earlier awaiting trial, and end when the defendant was released from prison. If the
defendant remained in prison due to another conviction, use the date on which the defendant was
exonerated as the release date. If the defendant was released on bond or was paroled and then
returned to prison, use the time between conviction and final release from prison (do not
subtract the time spend on bond, parole, etc).

For acquittals/dismissals, the time served should begin when the defendant was charged with the
relevant crimes and end when the defendant was released from prison. If the charge date is not
known, you may use the arrest date.

290
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Additional correctional control between time of release and exoneration (add_control):
nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #59] Correctional control refers to substantial restraints on the defendant’s freedom
(i.e., house arrest, GPS monitoring, curfews, registered sex offender, parole, etc.); it does NOT
include bail or bond.

Total amount of time under correctional/criminal justice control from conviction to
exoneration (total_cj): scale

[Question #59(b)] Write in. Measured in months. Round up days.
For erroneous convictions, the time should begin when the defendant was convicted of the
relevant crimes while for acquittals/dismissals, the time should begin when the defendant was
charged with the relevant crimes. If the charge date is not known, you may use the arrest date.
For both sets of cases, the point at which the defendant is said to leave the criminal justice
system with regards to the crime at issue is the point at which the government is no longer using
the criminal justice system to implicate the defendant in the crime. This question is NOT
concerned with whether the defendant is still in the criminal justice system for reasons that are
not related to the crime(s) at issue (this may be indicated in Q 62). In addition, when

291
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

determining the point of exit of the criminal justice system, do not take into consideration action
taken by the defendant in seeking compensation or bringing a civil suit. However, if a defendant
seeks a pardon after his conviction is vacated, the grant of a pardon may be considered the exit.

Defendant responsible for exoneration (def_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] General instructions for the following 18 variables: Put a “1” next to any
individual/group that is predominately responsible for the exoneration. We are mainly concerned
with the primary causes—these can be individuals or groups who are “but for” causes or those
who played crucial, direct, and active roles in the exoneration (e.g. an agency that takes the
initiative to retest forensic evidence or a witness who recants the crucial identification of the
defendant).
Put a “2” next to any individual/group who actively opposes the exoneration (rather than simply
supported the conviction in the first instance), particularly in the face of overwhelming evidence
of innocence. Both categories should be limited to only a few individuals/groups and should
generally NOT include individuals who are simply doing their job.

Victim/supposed victim responsible for exoneration (vic_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement

292
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Real culprit responsible for exoneration (real_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Witness responsible for exoneration (witness_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Police responsible for exoneration (police_resp): nominal

293
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above. Police refers to city, county, or state police, as
distinguished from federal law enforcement.

Convicting prosecutor responsible for exoneration (conv_prosec_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above. A prosecutor in one sense is always “opposed” to the
defendant in the initial trial stage, but do not note this unless there are facts to suggest that the
prosecutor afterward maintained that the defendant was guilty despite an official recognition of
innocence or actively sought to hinder the exoneration in some way. If there is a discrepancy
between the roles of the individual prosecutor and the office in the exoneration, choose the
official position of the District or State Attorney’s Office. [Note, this is different than the
instructions for Question #52, which instructs you to note the discrepancies only]

294
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Subsequent prosecutor responsible for exoneration (sub_prosec_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above. A prosecutor in one sense is always “opposed” to the
defendant in the initial trial stage, but do not note this unless there are facts to suggest that the
prosecutor afterward maintained that the defendant was guilty despite an official recognition of
innocence or actively sought to hinder the exoneration in some way. Prosecutor refers to both
the individual and the office. If there is a discrepancy between the roles of the individual
prosecutor and the office in the exoneration, choose the official position of the District or State
Attorney’s Office. [Note, this is different than the instructions for Question #56, which instructs
you to note the discrepancies only]

Judge responsible for exoneration (judge_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

295
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

State administrative office responsible for exoneration (state_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Federal law enforcement responsible for exoneration (fed_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Original/trial defense attorney responsible for exoneration (defense_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

296
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Subsequent (retrial or appellate) defense attorney responsible for exoneration
(sub_defense_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above. An appellate defense attorney should not be given a
“1” if (s)he was simply seeking the defendant’s exoneration in the normal course of employment.

Family of defendant responsible for exoneration (family_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Friend of defendant responsible for exoneration (friend_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed

297
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Journalist responsible for exoneration (jour_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Professor responsible for exoneration (prof_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

Innocence project or organization responsible for exoneration (IP_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible

298
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above. An innocence project or organization must be an
organization that is convened for the purpose of establishing the innocence of defendants
charged or convicted of crimes in the United States.

Other individual group responsible for exoneration (other_resp): nominal
0=no known involvement
1=predominantly responsible
2=actively opposed
99=not applicable (acquittal or dismissal)

[Question #56] See instructions above

PROSECUTION’S EVIDENCE

*For the next several pages of questions that correspond to Q#57 on the narrative coding sheet,
you should answer in reference to what was known to the trier of fact/decisionmaker, unless the
question explicitly mentions a trial. For the questions that do ask about a trial, answer “not
applicable” if there was no trial.

299
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

**If the case is an erroneous conviction, code only the information that was used at the first
conviction. If the case is an acquittal, code only the information that was used at the trial that
resulted in the acquittal.

Eyewitness identification of the defendant (identification): nominal
0=no
1=yes

This includes any informal or formal identification procedure used by law enforcement or
prosecution, including pre-trial procedures that led to an identification of the defendant.
Eyewitnesses include not only individuals who witnessed the defendant commit the crime but
also those who identify the defendant as someone who was at or near the scene of the crime.
However, it does not include, for instance, victims of past crimes who are allowed to testify as to
the defendant’s identity to establish motive or MO or witnesses who identify the defendant as
having been with the victim at some other time and place.

Eyewitness misidentification of the defendant (misid): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable/no identification made

[Question #57(d)(1)] Misidentification is genuinely mistaken identification of the defendant, as
opposed to perjured or willful false identifications, or correct identifications that nonetheless led

300
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

to an erroneous conviction. This refers to the eyewitnesses in general, so if there is one
eyewitness that misidentified the defendant (and one that did not), the answer is still “Yes”.

Victim 1 misidentification (vic1_misid): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (victim deceased)

[Question #57(d)(2-3)] This includes identifications that are genuinely mistaken, as well as
perjured testimony.

Victim 1 provided description of perpetrator prior to identification (vic1_descr): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (victim deceased)

[Question #57(d)(4)] This refers to some description of the facial features/body of the defendant
(rather than clothing).

Victim 1 provided details of a unique characteristic of the perpetrator (a tattoo, birthmark,
etc) (vic1_unique): nominal
0=no
1=yes

301
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

99=not applicable (victim deceased)

[Question #57(d)(4)] This includes tattoos, a gold tooth, a scar, bodily disfigurement.

Discrepancy between victim 1 description and person identified (vic1_discrepancy):
nominal
0=no
1=yes
99= not applicable (victim deceased)

[Question #57(d)(4)] This will require some subjective judgment. However, you may rely, in
your discretion, on the conclusions of those involved in the case (i.e., defense attorneys) as long
as there are sufficient details about the victim’s original description and the defendant’s
characteristics to support their conclusion. Of course, some minor discrepancy such as the
difference between being 5’8 and 5’10 is not sufficient.

Victim 1 pretrial identification 1 (vic1_id1): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant

302
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant
13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedure

[Question #53(d)(5)] This refers to the first time the victim was asked to identify the defendant,
when the defendant was present. Note that a show-up means the police or prosecutors presented
the defendant (or created a situation in which the defendant would be presented) to the
eyewitness to make an identification. A direct implication includes either the victim identifying
the defendant by name or other unique features (i.e., “my uncle’s girlfriend”), as well as the

303
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

victim pointing out the defendant independently of any request for identification (for instance,
the victim is walking down the street, sees a man, and realizes he is her rapist). Single photo
identifications include victims or other individuals who view the crime or the scene on
surveillance video and made an identification of the perpetrator.

Victim 1 pretrial identification 2 (vic1_id2): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant
5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant

304
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedure

[Question #53(d)(5)] This refers to the second time the victim was asked to identify the
defendant, when the defendant was present.

This is repeated for a third pre-trial identification.

Victim 1 made cross-racial identification (vic_cross_race1): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #53(d)(6)]

Victim 1 time elapsed between crime and first positive identification (vic_id_elapse1): scale

[Question #53(d)(7)] Time in days. If the ID was made on the same day, put a “0” (not a
decimal or a 1).

305
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Victim 1 certainty of identification (vic_cert_id1): nominal
0=not certain
1= certain
99= not applicable (victim deceased)

[Question #53(d)(8)] If multiple procedures were used, please specify the level of certainty for
the first positive ID. Thus, if the eyewitness failed to make an ID, this is not the same as being
uncertain. [Note, this is somewhat different than in the narrative coding sheet where you were
asked to note the level of certainty for each identification]. If you have a detailed account of the
identification and no mention is made of uncertainty, you may assume that it was certain.

Victim 1 took multiple tries to identify defendant (vic_id_multiple1): nominal
0=no (ID at first procedure)
1=yes (multiple tries before identification is made)
99=no identification procedure

[Question #53(d)(5)] Select “Yes” if the victim failed to identify the defendant once or multiple
times before making a positive ID. Select “No” if the victim made a positive ID in the first
identification procedure, even if the victim was not entirely certain of her identification. Do not
consider identification procedures that did not include the defendant (i.e., an array in which the
defendant was not present). If there is no mention of a failed attempt to make an ID, you may
assume “No”.

306
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Victim 1’s pretrial identification was presented at trial (vic_pretrial1): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no trial)

You may assume that is was presented at trial only if you have a detailed description of the ID
process and/or trial, in other words, a description that would mention if a pre-trial ID was
excluded by the court for some reason. If the victim failed to make an ID, you would need to
know more information before assuming that this failed attempt was or was not presented at
trial.

Victim 1 made misidentification at trial (vic_trial1): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no trial)

This may be assumed unless there is evidence to the contrary.

Victim 2 misidentified the defendant (vic2_misid): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99= not applicable (victim deceased or no 2nd victim)

307
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #53(d)(2-3)] See instructions above.

Victim 2 provided description of perpetrator prior to identification (vic2_descr): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99= not applicable (victim deceased)

[Question #57(d)(4)] See instructions above.

Victim 2 provided details of a unique characteristic of the perpetrator (a tattoo, birthmark,
etc) (vic2_unique): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99= not applicable (victim deceased)

[Question #57(d)(4)] See instructions above.

Discrepancy between victim 2 description and person identified (vic2_discrepancy):
nominal
0=no
1=yes
99= not applicable (victim deceased)

308
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #57(d)(4)] This will require some subjective judgment. However, you may rely, in
your discretion, on the conclusions of those involved in the case (i.e., defense attorneys) as long
as there are sufficient details about the victim’s original description and the defendant’s
characteristics to support their conclusion.

Victim 2 pretrial identification 1 (vic2_id1): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant
5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant

309
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedure

[Question #53(d)(5)] This refers to the first time the victim was asked to identify the defendant,
when the defendant was present.

Victim 2 pretrial identification 2 (vic2_id2): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant
5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present

310
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant
13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedure used

[Question #53(d)(5)] This refers to the second time the victim was asked to identify the
defendant, when the defendant was present.

Victim 2 made cross-racial identification (vic_cross_race2): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #53(d)(6)] See instructions above.

Victim 2 time elapsed between crime and first positive identification (vic_id_elapse2): scale

311
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #53(d)(7)] Write in, time in days.

Victim 2 certainty of identification (vic_cert_id2): nominal
0=not certain
1= certain
99= not applicable

[Question #53(d)(8)] See instructions above.

Victim 2 took multiple tries to identify defendant (vic_id_multiple2): nominal
0=no (ID at first procedure)
1=yes (multiple tries before identification is made)
99=not applicable

Select “Yes” if the victim failed to identify the defendant once or multiple times before making a
positive ID. Select “No” if the victim made a positive ID in the first identification procedure,
even if she was not certain of her identification. Do not consider identification procedures that
did not include the defendant (i.e., an array in which the defendant was not present).

Victim 2’s pretrial misidentification was presented at trial (vic_pretrial2): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no trial)

312
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Victim 2 made misidentification at trial (vic_trial2): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no trial)

Continue for victims #1-10

Discrepancy between any victim’s description and the defendant (Addvic_discrepancy):
nominal
0=no
1=yes

You will not need to enter this manually. Syntax will calculate whether there was a discrepancy
between any victim’s description of the perpetrator and the defendant’s appearance.

Unique details of perpetrator provided by any victim (Addvic_unique): nominal
0=no
1=yes

You will not need to enter this manually. Syntax will calculate whether victim gave a unique
description of the perpetrator.

313
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Eyewitness 1 misidentification (witness1): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #53(d)(2-3)]

Eyewitness 1 provided description of perpetrator prior to identification (witness1_descr):
nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no eyewitness)

[Question #57(d)(4)]

Eyewitness 1 provided details of a unique characteristic of the perpetrator (a tattoo,
birthmark, etc) (witness1_unique): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #57(d)(4)]

314
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Discrepancy between eyewitness 1 description and person identified
(witness1_discrepancy): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #57(d)(4)] This will require some subjective judgment. However, you may rely, in
your discretion, on the conclusions of those involved in the case (i.e., defense attorneys) as long
as there are sufficient details about the victim’s original description and the defendant’s
characteristics to support their conclusion.

Eyewitness 1 pretrial identification 1 (wit1_id1): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant
5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant

315
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant
13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedures used

[Question #57(d)(5)] This refers to the first time the victim was asked to identify the defendant,
when the defendant was present.

Eyewitness 1 pretrial identification 2 (wit1_id2): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant

316
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant
13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedures used

[Question #57(d)(5)] This refers to the second time the victim was asked to identify the
defendant, when the defendant was present.

Eyewitness 1 pretrial identification 3 (wit1_id3): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant

317
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant
5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant
13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedures used

318
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #57(d)(5)] This refers to the third time the victim was asked to identify the defendant,
when the defendant was present.

Eyewitness 1 made cross-racial identification (cross_race1): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #57(d)(6)]

Eyewitness 1 time elapsed between crime and first positive identification (id_elapse1): scale

[Question #57(d)(7) Write in, time in days

Eyewitness 1 certainty of identification (cert_id1): nominal.
0=not certain
1= certain
99=not applicable

[Question #57(d)(8)] see above

Eyewitness 1 took multiple tries to identify defendant (id_multiple1): nominal
0=no (ID at first procedure)

319
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=yes (multiple tries before identification is made)
99=not applicable

Select “Yes” if the eyewitness failed to identify the defendant once or multiple times before
making a positive ID. Select “No” if the eyewitness made a positive ID in the first identification
procedure, even if she was not certain about her identification. Do not consider identification
procedures that did not include the defendant (i.e., an array in which the defendant was not
present).

Eyewitness 1’s pretrial misidentification was presented at trial (witness_pretrial1): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no trial)

Eyewitness 1 made misidentification at trial (witness_trial1): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no trial)

See instructions above

Eyewitness 2 misidentification (witness2): nominal
0=no

320
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=yes

[Question #57(d)(2-3)] See instructions above

Eyewitness 2 provided description of perpetrator prior to identification (witness2_descr):
nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no eyewitness)

[Question #57(d)(4)] See instructions above

Eyewitness 2 provided details of a unique characteristic of the perpetrator (a tattoo,
birthmark, etc) (witness2_unique): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #57(d)(4)] See instructions above

Discrepancy between eyewitness 2 description and person identified
(witness2_discrepancy): nominal
0=no

321
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #57(d)(4)] This will require some subjective judgment. However, you may rely, in
your discretion, on the conclusions of those involved in the case (i.e., defense attorneys) as long
as there are sufficient details about the victim’s original description and the defendant’s
characteristics to support their conclusion.

Eyewitness 2 pretrial identification 1 (wit2_id1): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant
5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant

322
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant
13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedures used

[Question #57(d)(5)] This refers to the first time the victim was asked to identify the defendant,
when the defendant was present.

Eyewitness 2 pretrial identification 2 (wit2_id2): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant
5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present

323
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant
13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedures used

[Question #57(d)(5)] This refers to the second time the victim was asked to identify the
defendant, when the defendant was present.

Eyewitness 2 pretrial identification 3 (wit2_id3): nominal
0=witness volunteers identification of the defendant
1=show-up used, did not identify the defendant when defendant was present
2=show-up used, identified the defendant
3=sequential line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
4= sequential line-up used, identified the defendant

324
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

5=simultaneous line-up used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
6=simultaneous line-up used, identified the defendant
7=unknown line-up procedure, did not identify defendant when the defendant was present
8=unknown line-up procedure, identified the defendant
9=sequential photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
10=sequential photo array used, identified the defendant
11=simultaneous photo array used, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was
present
12=simultaneous photo array used, identified the defendant
13=unknown photo array procedure used, did not identify the defendant when the
defendant was present
14=unknown photo array procedure used, identified the defendant
15=single photo, did not identify the defendant when the defendant was present
16=single photo, identified the defendant
99=no identification procedures used

[Question #57(d)(5)] This refers to the third time the victim was asked to identify the defendant,
when the defendant was present.

Eyewitness 2 made cross-racial identification (cross_race2): nominal
0=no

325
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=yes
99=not applicable

[Question #57(d)(6)]

Eyewitness 2 time elapsed between crime and identification (id_elapse2): scale

[Question #57(d)(7)] Write in, time in days.

Eyewitness 2 certainty of identification (cert_id2): nominal
0=not certain
1= certain
99=not applicable

[Question #57(d)(8)] see above

Eyewitness 2 took multiple tries to identify the defendant (id_multiple2): nominal
0=no (only one try needed)
1=yes (multiple tries required)
99=not applicable (no identification procedures conducted)

Select “Yes” if the eyewitness failed to identify the defendant once or multiple times before
making a positive ID. Select “No” if the eyewitness made a positive ID in the first identification

326
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

procedure, even if she was uncertain as to her identification. Do not consider identification
procedures that did not include the defendant (i.e., an array in which the defendant was not
present).

Eyewitness 2’s pretrial misidentification was presented at trial (witness_pretrial2): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no trial)

Eyewitness 2 made misidentification at trial (witness_trial2): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no trial)

Continue for Eyewitnesses #1-10

Eyewitness(es) testify at trial (witness_testify): nominal
0=no (did not testify)
1=yes (testified)
99=not applicable (no eyewitness)

[Question #57(d)(10)] This is a comprehensive variable; thus if there is at least one eyewitness
(including victim) testifying as to a misidentification at trial, select “Yes.”

327
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Eyewitness(es) pre-trial identification presented at trial (witness_trial): nominal
0=evidence of identifications not presented at trial
1=evidence of eyewitness identifications presented at trial
99=not applicable (no eyewitness)

[Question #57(d)(11) Again, this is a comprehensive variable. So if at least one pre-trial
misidentification was presented at trial, select “Yes”. Presented at trial means either that the
eyewitness testified to the prior identification or the identification itself was entered into
evidence (i.e., a photo array with the defendant’s picture initialed.)

Prosecution witness(es) provided exculpatory testimony/evidence for the defendant
(exulp_wit): nominal
0=no
1=yes

This variable includes both eyewitnesses and noneyewitnesses who provide exculpatory evidence
in favor of the defendant and who make up part of the prosecution’s case.

Non-eyewitness(es) provided testimony against the defendant (noneye_test): nominal
0=no
1=yes

328
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

This does NOT include criminal justice personnel (such as police officers), medical personnel, or
expert witnesses; instead the focus here is on “lay” witnesses. For example: friends or jailmates
who supposedly hear the defendant bragging about the crime, the victim’s husband who testifies
he left his wife at a certain time before the crime and she was in good spirit, etc. While there is
another variable that specifically records falsehoods by a jailhouse informant, jailhouse
informants that are non-eyewitnesses should be included here as well.

Non-eyewitness(es) recanted testimony (noneye_recant): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no non-eyewitness)

If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is made of recantation,
you may assume the witness did not recant.

Victim(s) testified at trial (vic_testify): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable/deceased/no trial

[Question #57 (a)] You may assume the victim testified at trial where it is reasonable to do so,
i.e., at a rape trial.

329
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Additional misidentified information (add_misid): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #57(e)] This includes an eyewitness who testifies (erroneously) to seeing the
defendant’s car, clothing, etc. at or near the scene of the crime, or before or after the crime took
place. It also includes identification through other senses, such as smell or voice. If a non-victim
or non-eyewitness identifies a sketch as representing or resembling the defendant, this can also
be included. This can include perjured testimony. However, it does not include testimony that is
correct (i.e ., the witness really did see the defendant near the scene of the crime) but was
nonetheless misleading or improperly used.

Victim recanted identification prior to trial (vic_prior_recantid): nominal
0=did not recant
1=recanted
99=not applicable/deceased (no identification given)

[Question #57(d)(11)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is
made of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Victim recanted identification during trial (vic_trial_recantid): nominal
0=did not recant
1=recanted

330
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

99=not applicable/deceased (no identification given or no trial)

[Question #57(d)(11)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is
made of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Victim recanted identification after trial (vic_later_recantid): nominal
0=did not recant
1=recanted
99=not applicable/deceased (no identification given or no trial)

[Question #57(d)(11)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is
made of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Victim recanted crime prior to trial (vic_prior_recantcrime): nominal
0=did not recant
1=recanted
99=not applicable/deceased (no testimony given)

[Question #57(b)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is made
of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Victim recanted crime during trial (vic_trial_recantcrime): nominal
0=did not recant

331
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=recanted
99=not applicable/deceased (no testimony given or no trial)

[Question #57(b)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is made
of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Victim recanted crime after trial (vic_later_recantcrime): nominal
0=did not recant
1=recanted
99=not applicable/deceased (no testimony given or no trial)

[Question #57(b)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is made
of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Victim intentionally misidentified the defendant (vic_perjury): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged
99=not applicable

This includes lying to officials in an investigation, even when there is no trial or hearing (and
thus no official perjury).

332
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Continue for victims #2-10

Eyewitness 1 (non-victim) recanted identification prior to trial (wit1_prior_recantid):
nominal
0=did not recant
1=recanted
99=not applicable (no identification given)

[Question #57(d)(11)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is
made of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Eyewitness 1 (non-victim) recanted identification during trial (wit1_trial_recantid):
nominal
0=did not recant
1=recanted
99=not applicable (no identification given or no trial)

[Question #57(d)(11)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is
made of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Eyewitness 1 (non-victim) recanted identification after trial (wit1_later_recantid): nominal
0=did not recant
1=recanted

333
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

99=not applicable (no identification given or no trial)

[Question #57(d)(11)] If relatively detailed account of the testimony is given and no mention is
made of recantation, you may assume the witness did not recant.

Any eyewitness (including victim) recanted the identification or crime before or during
trial (Addrecant): nominal
0=no
1=yes

You will not need to enter this manually.

Eyewitness 1 (non-victim) intentionally misidentified the defendant (wit1_perjury): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged
99=not applicable

[Question #57(g)] This includes lying to officials in an investigation, even when there is no trial
or hearing (and thus no official perjury).

Continue for Eyewitnesses #2-10

334
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Intentional misidentification by any victim or eyewitness (Intentional_MisID): nominal
0=no
1=yes

You will not need to enter this manually.

Intentionally false testimony by jailhouse informant/snitch (informant): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(g)] A jailhouse informant is an individual, already imprisoned or with a court
case pending/threatened, who informs the police, prosecutors, or other officials of the activities
and words of the defendant. A snitch is an individual who gives information to the police for the
return of a favor—monetary or otherwise. If there was no jailhouse informant or snitch deceit,
answer “no”. Note, if there is an informant or snitch who testimony appears reliable (not false),
do not mark “yes” for this variable. Instead, simply record this witness as a non-eyewitness.
This includes lying to officials in an investigation, even when there is no trial or hearing (and
thus no official perjury).

Perjury or lying by criminal justice officials (police, prosecutors, etc) (cj_perjury): nominal
0=no
1=yes

335
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

2=alleged

[Question #57(h)] This includes lying to officials in an investigation, even when there is no trial
or hearing (and thus no official perjury).

Non-eyewitness(es) committed perjury/gave intentionally false testimony (noneye_perjury):
nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged
99=not applicable

This includes lying to officials in an investigation, even when there is no trial or hearing (and
thus no official perjury).

False confession (confession): nominal
0=no confession/statements
1=false incriminating statements made
2=falsely confessed to the crime.

[Question #57(f)]

336
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Evidence of physical or threat of physical abuse to elicit false confession or incriminating
statements (abuse_false): nominal
0=no abuse indicated
1=physical abuse or threat thereof
99=no false statements made

[Question #57(f)]Physical abuse refers to bodily harm inflicted on the defendant, such as
beating, knocking down, or depriving the person of sleep or food. A threat of physical abuse
must be imminent and direct from the police or prosecutors; indirect threats, such as the threat
of a life sentence if convicted or the suggestion that the defendant will be raped by inmates in
prison, does not count.
This includes credible allegations of physical abuse (such as cases where there is a history of
physical abuse in the police department, the defendant wins a large settlement from the city, or
the court says there is enough evidence of the allegation to continue the civil suit).

Significant Forensic evidence presented (foren_evid): nominal
0=presented, no errors
1=presented, errors present
99=not presented

[Question #57(j)] Forensic evidence is “forensic science” evidence—evidence that has been
evaluated using scientific methods. If the case is an erroneous conviction or rightful acquittal,

337
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

this question asks what was presented a trial; if the case is an erroneous plea or rightful
dismissal, then this question refers to forensics presented up unto the point of plea or dismissal.
The presence of semen, tears, or bruising should be marked as “forensic evidence presented,” if
we know it played a role in the case (i.e., verified victim’s claim that she was raped). If there is
no mention of forensics, mark a “99”; however, we acknowledge that there is a high likelihood
in almost all murder and rape cases there will be some forensic evidence, which we might not be
aware of (not mentioned in sources), to prove that the supposed crime did occur. Specifically,
cases from the Innocence Project must have had forensic evidence collected in order for the
defendant’s convictions to be vacated via DNA testing. But again, if we are not aware of this
evidence playing any role in the original convictions or dismissal/acquittal, mark ‘99’.

This is an umbrella variable. The next eight variables drill down on the specific type(s) of
forensic evidence presented.

Impression/print evidence presented (impression): nominal
0=presented, no errors
1=presented, errors present
99=not presented

[Question #57(j)] This includes all types of impression evidence: fingerprints, palmprints, shoe
impressions, etc. It also includes errors that are the result of fraud.

338
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Ballistics evidence presented (ballistics): nominal
0=presented, no errors
1=presented, errors present
99=not presented

[Question #57(j)] It includes errors that are the result of fraud.

Microscopic hair evidence presented (hair): nominal
0=presented, no errors
1=presented, errors present
99=not presented

[Question #57(j)] This includes other fibers, as well as hair. In determining whether there was
an error in the hair evidence presented, use the FBI guidelines [see Forensic Science
Communications, Forensic Hair Comparison Review Article, 11:2, April 2009]. According to the
guidelines as well as some case law on the subject, it is almost certainly error if the expert
testifies about percentages or the specific likelihood of a “match”. This kind of information is
not known for hair comparison (in contrast, say, to DNA testing). It is likely NOT error if the
expert merely says that the hair is consistent with, or can be associated with, the defendant (or in
reverse, that the defendant can be excluded as the source of the hair).
It also includes errors that are the result of fraud.

Serology evidence presented (serology): nominal

339
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

0=presented, no errors in serology evidence (analysis or testimony)
1=presented, errors in testing
2=presented, errors in testimony
3=presented, errors in testing and testimony
99=no serology evidence

[Question #57(j)] It includes errors that are the result of fraud.

DNA evidence (dna): nominal
0=presented, no errors
1=presented, errors present
99=not presented

[Question #57(j)] It includes errors that are the result of fraud.

Bite mark evidence (bite_mark): nominal
0=presented, no errors
1=presented, errors present
99=not presented

[Question #57(j)] It includes errors that are the result of fraud.

340
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Medical examiner or other medical error (med_error): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #57(j)]This includes erroneous diagnoses of cause of death, disease, etc., as well as
errors made in the administration or analysis of a rape kit. It also includes errors that are the
result of fraud.

Exculpatory forensic evidence presented by prosecution (forensic_exculp): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=no forensic evidence presented

This includes evidence presented up through the trial, plea, or dismissal. Exculpatory evidence is
material evidence that tends to show the defendant did not commit the crime. Thus, latent
fingerprints on the murder weapon or O type blood found in a rape victim that do NOT match
the defendant or the victim would be exculpatory. Blood found at the scene that does not match
the defendant but does match the victim would not be exculpatory (though it is also not
inculpatory) because it does not exclude the defendant in any meaningful way. Likewise, latent
prints that do not match the defendant but are found at a crime scene that is a public place, such
as a store, are probably not exculpatory, unless these prints are located specifically in areas that
we know the perpetrator touched.

341
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Exculpatory DNA evidence presented by prosecution (DNA_exculp): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=no forensic evidence presented

If you mark “yes” for this variable, you should have also marked “yes” for the previous
variable, forensic_exculp.

Forensic fraud evident (foren_fraud): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged
99=no forensic evidence presented

[Question #57(j)] This includes fraud in either forensic testing or testimony. As with all
subsequent “alleged”, there must be a clear basis for the allegation and the allegation must be
sufficiently serious to have possibly influenced the outcome of the trial.

Exculpatory physical evidence presented by prosecution (physical_exculp): nominal
0=no
1=yes

342
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

This includes exculpatory physical evidence (other than forensics) that is discovered and/or
presented by the prosecution. It includes text messages and cell phone calls that are exculpatory
on their face, without scientific investigation (i.e., forensic analysis).

Anonymous tip/information received by police/prosecutors (anonymous): nominal
0=no
1=yes

This only includes information that implicates the defendant or establishes facts of the crime
(rather than a tip that exonerates the defendant). This was not directly asked on the narrative
coding sheet, so you will have to imply the information from what is provided or do more
research to obtain an answer.

Surveillance tape or wiretap evidence that implicates the defendant (surveillance): nominal
0=no
1=yes

This only includes surveillance or recordings that implicates the defendant in the crime (rather
than exonerate). This was not directly asked on the narrative coding sheet, so you will have to
imply the information from what is provided or do more research to obtain an answer.

Recovery of victim’s stolen property from/or near defendant (stolen_prop): nominal
0=no

343
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=yes

This was not directly asked on the narrative coding sheet, so you will have to imply the
information from what is provided or do more research to obtain an answer. If there is no record
of the victim having property stolen or no record of the victim’s property being found in the
vicinity of the defendant, answer “no”. This can be your default choice unless you have positive
information to the contrary.

Withholding of exculpatory evidence (withheld_exculp): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(k)] This is essentially a Brady violation. It refers primarily to evidence withheld
by police or prosecutors. If information was withheld by a crime lab, mark it as “forensic
fraud”, above. If you answer ‘yes’, the following two variables require you to select who was
responsible for the withholding (police, prosecutor, or both). If you answered ‘no’, then select a
‘99’ for the next two variables.

Evidence withheld by police (police_withheld): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

344
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

99=not applicable (no evidence withheld)

[Question #57(k)]

Evidence withheld by prosecutor (prosec_withheld): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged
99= not applicable (no evidence withheld)

[Question #57(k)]

Police Error (police_error): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(l)] This is a mistake or omission that is made by the police and does not imply
intentional or extremely negligent wrongdoing. It includes forgetting to collect or loosing certain
evidence, suggestive identification procedures that do not violate a defendant’s rights, and
sloppy interrogations where information is unintentionally leaked to the victim or defendant.

345
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Prosecutor Error (prosec_error): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(k)] This is a mistake or omission that is made by the prosecution and does not
imply intentional or extremely negligent wrongdoing. It includes forgetting to collect or loosing
certain evidence, suggestive identification procedures that do not violate a defendant’s rights,
and sloppy interrogations where information is unintentionally leaked to the victim or defendant.

Judicial Error (judge_error): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #53(k)] This is a mistake or omission that does not imply intentional wrongdoing. It
includes errors that are reversed by a higher court upon appeal.

Juror Error (juror_error): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged
99=not applicable, no jury

346
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #57(k)] This is a mistake or omission that does not imply intentional wrongdoing

Other Criminal Justice Official Error (off_error): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(k)] This is a mistake or omission that does not imply intentional wrongdoing. It
includes actions by state officials such as hospital or prison administrators, state forensic
psychologist, or Child Protective Services.

Police Misconduct (police_miscon): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(m)] There must be intentionality or extreme negligence that is either present or
can be legally inferred. It includes intentional Brady violations, as well coercive interrogation
tactics where the defendant later wins a civil suit against the officers. Please note, however, that
police are usually allowed to lie to defendants in interrogation or use other forms of
psychological pressure, so do not mark this as error or misconduct, unless there is strong

347
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

evidence to indicate that in this case, it was deemed unacceptable (i.e., mentally retarded
defendant gets his conviction overturned based on a coerced confession)

Prosecutor Misconduct (prosec_miscon): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(k)] There must be intentionality or extreme negligence that is either present or
can be legally inferred. It includes intentional Brady violations and instances where the
defendant wins a malicious prosecution civil suit.

Judicial Misconduct (judge_miscon): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(k)] There must be intentionality or extreme negligence that is either present or
can be legally inferred.

Juror Misconduct (juror_miscon): nominal
0=no
1=yes

348
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

2=alleged
99=not applicable, no jury

[Question #57(k)] There must be intentionality or extreme negligence that is either present or
can be legally inferred.

Other Criminal Justice Official Misconduct (other_cj_miscon): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(k)] There must be intentionality or extreme negligence that is either present or
can be legally inferred. This includes coercive interrogation techniques or a fraudulent
investigation by Child Protective Services, the US Postal Service, a state forensic psychologist,
and other government officials.

Criminal Justice Action/Success Demanded by Community (conv_demand): nominal
0=no
1=yes

[Question #57(k)] This should only be selected when there is a clear indication that this was a
substantial factor in the investigation or conviction of the defendant. It can be based on either a
direct statement by a criminal justice official (i.e. “we were under a lot of pressure to solve this

349
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

crime because…”) or on a post hoc interpretation by the media, scholars, or ourselves, verified
by the presence of special indicators. These indicators include: the creation of a special
commission to deal with the crime(s), the horrific nature or number of crimes (i.e., a series of
child rapes), a highly publicized crime, a rushed investigation and speedy trial, and a crime
involving racial tensions in a community. The indicators by themselves are not enough to answer
‘yes’. Pressure from elected officials (i.e. District Attorney, Mayor, etc.) to solve a crime can
constitute community outrage.

Misleading circumstantial evidence (misleading_circ): nominal
0=no
1=yes

Circumstantial evidence is opposed to direct evidence (like an eyewitness who says she saw the
crime occur). Circumstantial evidence is also generally distinct from confessions and expert
testimony. In keeping with this, you should NOT include forensic evidence like serology that
merely proved that the defendant could have committed the crime. This question is therefore
really getting at whether there were coincidences or suspicious circumstances that falsely led the
police or prosecutors to believe the defendant committed the crime. Examples include: the
defendant has the same name as the real perpetrator, or he drives an identical car; the defendant
was known to have visited the victim the night of her death and had threatened her.

Incompetent defense attorney: nominal
0=no

350
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=yes
2=alleged

[Question #57(u)] Answer “yes” if there is an official court finding of ineffective assistance of
counsel or some other official recognition of attorney incompetence (i.e., the defendant won a
civil suit against the attorney). You may also indicate that defense counsel was incompetent if
you have direct, clear evidence of the counsel’s errors/misconduct. However, if in doubt, select
“alleged”.

DEFENSE’S EVIDENCE

Defense put on a defense (def_presented): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=yes, but no trial (plea or dismissal)

[Question #57(w)] In dismissal cases, select “no” if the defendant fails to present any material
evidence against his charge(s). In cases that go to trial, select “no” if the defense rested after the
prosecution’s case.

Defendant testified at trial (def_testify): nominal
0=defendant did not testify

351
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

1=defendant testified
99= not applicable/no trial

[Question #57(c)]

Family member is witness for defense (family_witness): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no defense)

[Question #57(w)] Family includes spouses and significant others.

Friend is witness for defense (friends_witness): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no defense)

[Question #57(w)]

Non-family or non-friend is witness for defense (other_witness): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no defense)

352
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #57(w)] Includes police officers if called for exculpatory reasons. It also includes
bosses and co-workers, unless these witnesses are clearly also friends or family.

Number of non-expert witnesses called (defense_witness): scale

[Question #57(w)] Write in. Total witnesses.

Alibi corroborated by physical evidence (phys_alibi): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no defense)

This is evidence other than testimony, such as phone records, timesheets, surveillance videos,
etc.

Defense had expert as witness (def_expert): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no defense)

[Question #57(w)]

353
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Number of expert witnesses called by the defense (number_experts): scale

[Question #57(w)] Write in. If you do not know the exact number, use the following answers:
96=a few, 3 or less; 97=more than 3

Defense offered testimony/evidence about mental illness/disorder or intellectual disability
(def_cognitive): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no defense)

[Question #57(w)] This evidence need not be presented by an expert (for example, a police
officer or family member may testify as to the defendant’s behavior on the night in question, or
about the medication that the defendant was on).

Exculpatory evidence presented by defense (exculp_presented): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no defense)

[Question #57] This was not directly asked on the coding sheet. It refers to favorable and
material evidence that is offered by the defense that tends to show the defendant did not commit
the crime in question. Material means that it is likely it could affect the outcome of an issue or

354
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

case; in other words, it is significant evidence. In general, alibi evidence and evidence of another
independent perpetrator will be exculpatory evidence, while character testimony will not. Other
types of evidence, such as insanity or police misconduct, will only be exculpatory if it directly
bears on whether the defendant committed the crime, rather than on an abuse of rights or the
degree of offense (murder vs. manslaughter, for instance).

Defense presented DNA evidence (DNA_defense): nominal
0=no
1=yes
99=not applicable (no defense)

Defense presented evidence of official misconduct (def_evid_misconduct): nominal
0=evidence not presented
1=evidence presented
99=not applicable (no defense)

[Question #57(w)] This only includes evidence or testimony of something egregious and
intentional, such as that the defendant was beaten or threatened into confessing; do NOT include
merely an argument made by defense counsel that the photo lineup was suggestive unless
evidence is presented that misconduct accompanied this lineup, such that, for instance, the
defense is able to have the lineup excluded

355
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

Defense presented evidence implicating another individual (def_other_ind): nominal
0=evidence not presented
1=evidence presented
99=not applicable (no defense)

[Question #57(w)] This refers to evidence that a specific individual (either by name or through
specific identifying features) committed the crime instead of the defendant, i.e., presenting an
eyewitness who says that Johnny, not the defendant, shot the gun. Or evidence that Johnny (not
the defendant) hated the victim, had a motive for murder, and bought poison right before the
victim died of an overdose.

Evidence provided by defense (def_evidence): string

Write in.

COMPENSATION

Compensation received (compensation): nominal
0=no
1=yes
2=currently seeking, not yet decided

356
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

[Question #60; Note, on the narrative coding sheet, there is not a separate category for
“currently seeking”]

Compensation amount (comp_amnt): scale, amount received in dollars

[Question #60] If there were multiple sources of compensation, add them together for a total
amount. If you do not have an exact amount and you can estimate, use that. However, if you
can’t estimate, leave blank. This is only for compensation actually received; if compensation is
pending, do not record it here.

POLICE FOUNDATION SCALE

Strength of suspect characteristics in indicating guilt (strength_suspect): ordinal
0=no suspect characteristics
1=weak
2=probative
3=highly probative

Strength of victim characteristics in suggesting defendant’s guilt (vic_strength): ordinal
1=weak
2=probative
3=highly probative

357
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

99=not applicable (victim deceased)

Strength of witness characteristics in suggesting defendant’s guilt (wit_strength): ordinal
1=weak
2=probative
3=highly probative
99=not applicable (no witness)

Strength of identification information in suggesting defendant’s guilt (id_strength): ordinal
1=weak
2=probative
3=highly probative
99=not applicable (no identification)

Strength of statements provided by defendant (statement_strength): ordinal
1=weak
2=probative
3=highly probative
99=not applicable (no statements)

Strength of physical evidence case (physical_strength): ordinal
1=weak
2=probative

358
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

3=highly probative
99=not applicable (no physical evidence)

Total strength of prosecution’s case (case_strength): ordinal
1=weak
2=probative
3=highly probative

Strength of evidence provided by defense in suggesting innocence (def_strength): ordinal
0=no defense
1=weak
2=probative
3=highly probative

359
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.F. Sample Factors and Exemplars from the Modified Police Foundation Strength of
Evidence Rating Scale

Type of Physical

1

2

3

Evidence

Weak

Probative

Highly Probative



Sample from
the victim is
mixed and
suspect cannot
be excluded.



Suspect has
blood type that
is the same as
that found in
seminal fluid
from the
victim’s PERK
kit



Suspect has
very rare blood
type or disease,
and traces of
which are found
in the seminal
fluid from the
victim’s PERK
kit.



Stolen items are
recovered in
garbage can.
Inscribed piece
of jewelry was
recovered.
Recovered
stolen laptop is
identified with
serial number.



Stolen item
found in
suspect’s
girlfriend’s car.



Stolen items are
found in
suspect’s locker
at work.
Victim’s
driver’s license
is on suspect.
Class ring is
found on
victim’s body
and matches
suspect’s
history.

Serology29




Recovery of Stolen
Items30




                                                            
29

This factor and the accompanying exemplars were developed by project researchers to supplement the original
Police Foundation scale.
30
This factor and the accompanying exemplars were modified from the original Police Foundation scale to
accommodate the project’s 3-point rating scale (original scale was 5-point).

360
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.G. Sample Case Narratives31
VIII.G.1. Erroneous Conviction Exemplar: Highly Probative Prosecution and Weak Defense

Harry Isaiah Sampson

The Crime

On the night of September 1, 1983, a 24-year-old cosmetician student left a friend’s house in
Irvine, California, and began walking towards a bus stop. As she was walking, an AfricanAmerican male in a Ford pulled up next to the woman and asked if she needed a ride. The
woman agreed, entered the car, and gave the driver directions to her home. Instead of following
her directions, however, the driver pulled a handgun on the victim and drove into a vacant lot.
The man then proceeded to rape the woman on the hood of the Ford, in the backseat of the car,
and again in the vacant lot.

After this assault, the man drove the victim to a house in the Sherwood community of Irvine. The
perpetrator took the victim inside, where he raped her again. The victim was then able to
convince her attacker to take her to a friend’s apartment. Once they neared the friend’s
apartment, the victim ran inside and locked the perpetrator out. Her friend called the Irvine
police to report the sexual assault, and the assailant fled the scene upon hearing the friend call
authorities.
                                                            
31

The following six examples of case narratives were rated using the modified Police Foundation strength of
evidence scale. All identifying information, including people, places, and specific dates, have been changed. Years
are true to life.

361
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

When the police arrived at the friend’s apartment, the victim gave a description of her attacker.
She stated the man was an African-American male with a short afro and short facial hair. She
said the assailant had confided to her that he had recently been released after 11 years in prison
and stated his name was Ronald. The victim gave a detailed description of the perpetrator’s
clothing, saying he was wearing blue jeans and a red pullover shirt with black or grey horizontal
stripes. She also gave the police a description of the perpetrator’s Ford, including the license
plate number.

In addition, the victim attempted to describe the Sherwood home where she was attacked. She
told police that while the perpetrator was driving her to the home, she was able to see the names
of two streets. The victim also gave a description of the house’s interior, including the presence
of Bud Light beer in the refrigerator and a magnum revolver in the perpetrator’s bedroom.
Furthermore, she asserted that the man had a key to the house.

The Identification

Police asked the victim to ride with them as they attempted to reconstruct where the perpetrator
had taken the victim. Following the victim’s memory of the route and the two street names she
remembered, the police ended near the house of John Sampson. Police immediately saw that
there was a Ford Escort parked in front of the Sampson house that fit the victim’s description of
the perpetrator’s car. The license plate of the parked Ford was only one digit different than the
number the victim remembered from the perpetrator’s plates. When officers walked up to the car,

362
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

one of them declared the hood was still warm, indicating it had been driven recently. The victim
said this was the house where she had been attacked.

The police officers entered the house and found John Sampson’s son, Harry Isaiah Sampson, in
the back room. Harry Isaiah Sampson, a 31-year-old African-American male on parole for armed
robbery, matched the physical description given by the victim. The clothes on the floor of
Sampson’s room were also similar to the victim’s description. Additionally, there was a magnum
revolver in Sampson’s desk and Bud Light in the refrigerator, as the victim had described.

According to Sampson’s account of the events, police conducted a show-up at the Sampson
home, and the victim positively identified Harry Isaiah Sampson as her attacker. This would
have occurred within several hours of the rape. However, as described below, at trial the state
maintained that the victim was not asked to make a pre-trial identification.

Given how well the victim’s recollection matched the evidence found at the Sampson home,
police immediately arrested Harry Isaiah Sampson for sexual assault. The victim was then taken
to the Glenmont Hospital and a rape kit examination was conducted.

The Trial

On December 22, 1983, Harry Isaiah Sampson was indicted in Orange County on charges of
aggravated sexual assault with a deadly weapon. The trial was held on January 17, 1984 in the
Judicial District Court in Orange County. At trial, the victim testified to her account of the sexual

363
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

assault and identified Sampson with certainty as her attacker. However, the victim specifically
stated she had not seen Sampson in person or in a photograph before the trial. Furthermore, the
arresting officer testified that after the victim pointed out the house where she had been raped, he
parked the car away from the house, and the victim did not see Sampson.

The state also called a doctor from Glenmont Hospital to testify to the rape examination. The
doctor stated that the vaginal area was consistent with recent trauma and that semen was present.
Although Sampson did not testify in his own defense, three witnesses were presented, including
his brother, father, and Amanda Jones, a renter in the Sampson house. Harry’s father, John
Sampson, testified that he was not in the house when the crime supposedly occurred; however,
he returned home that evening and did not notice anything unusual. He also confirmed that a gun
was kept in the drawer of the desk in the back room but stated that the weapon belonged to him,
not his son. He said that he and his son were sharing the back room as a bedroom. Harry’s
brother also testified that he was not there at the exact time of the crime but had not seen
anything unusual when he returned home. Amanda Jones, on the other hand, said she was home
the night the incident took place, sleeping in another part of the house. She said that Sampson
was in the home as well, and she did not hear or see anything suspicious.32 The defense did not
challenge the recollection of the arresting officer and victim about the pre-trial identification of
Sampson.33
                                                            
32

For reasons that are not clear, all three defense witnesses also testified to the layout of the house in which Harry
Isaiah Sampson was arrested. One reason for this may have been to show that the layout of the home, which the
victim attested to, was a typical house layout and not unusual in that neighborhood (i.e., the victim could easily have
been describing any number of other homes). Another reason for this testimony would be to point out discrepancies
between the victim’s description of the house where she was raped and the actual layout of the Sampson family
home.
33
Post-conviction, Harry Isaiah Sampson did contest the victim’s and arresting officer’s recollection of the day of
the crime. Sampson’s assertion that the victim identified him in a show-up was affirmed by two separate police
reports and a sworn affidavit of the case detective, Anthony Saunders.

364
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

On January 19, 1984, after deliberating for an hour and two minutes, the jury convicted Sampson
of aggravated sexual assault. On the same day, Sampson was sentenced to life in the California
Department of Correction by the jury.

STOP:

Please answer #1-4 on the case evaluation sheet before reading the
remainder of the narrative.

365
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

RESUME:

Now please continue to read about the exoneration and answer #5-8 on the
case evaluation sheet.

Post-Conviction

The California Supreme Court affirmed his conviction on direct appeal on August 2, 1991. While
incarcerated, Sampson worked to free himself, reading law books at night and drawing up legal
motions pro se. In September 2001, he filed a six-page, handwritten petition for a DNA test. It
took nearly four years before his request was heard, but in 2005 Sampson was granted DNA
testing on evidence collected from the rape kit. His DNA was compared to the spermatozoa
found on the vaginal swab and was excluded as the source. The victim confirmed that only the
rapist could be the source of the DNA, stating she did not have any partners at the time that could
have been the source of the semen.

In April 2006, Sampson was released from prison, pending the resolution of his motion for a writ
of habeas corpus with the California Supreme Court. On October 1, 2006, this court granted
relief on the grounds that the post-conviction DNA evidence excluded Sampson as the source of
the semen, and Sampson’s conviction and sentence were vacated. On December 3, 2006
California Governor Allen Allison issued a full pardon for Sampson on the basis of innocence.
Allison cited the DNA evidence in Sampson’s case as playing a pivotal role in his decision.

366
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

Harry Isaiah Sampson received $500,000 for his incarceration from the state’s compensation
fund for individuals who have been wrongfully convicted.

.

367
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.G.2. Erroneous Conviction Exemplar: Probative Prosecution and No Defense

Alex Borrensa

The Crime
Monique and Peter Zinfani, a Caucasian couple in their late 20s or early 30s, owned a diamond
importing business in Manchester, NH. There, they operated a private showroom from their
heavily secured home. The home’s security features, installed after an earlier robbery attempt,
included a front door identification camera, a buzz-in system, alarms, and even a safe room.
Despite these precautions, on January 15, 1980, the Zinfanis were robbed.

The perpetrators entered the showroom by first abducting the Zinfanis’ mail carrier, Lisa
Hauptmann. Hauptmann reported that a Caucasian man, “slim in build, thin through the face, . . .
about five foot seven inches in height,” and wearing a trench coat, approached her as she was on
her daily mail route. The man held a gun to Hauptmann’s back and ordered her into her mail
truck. Once there, the man stripped off her uniform, bound her, and put her into the back of the
truck. Hauptmann reported that a second Caucasian man, “around five foot seven or eight inches
tall and of medium build with dark brown hair,” also entered the truck.

The pair then drove the mail truck (with Hauptmann still in the back) to the Zinfanis’ home and
parked it outside the house. One of the men put on some parts of Hauptmann’s uniform and

368
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

slipped out of the truck with a large package; his accomplice followed. Left alone in the truck,
Hauptmann eventually freed herself and ran to get help.

Meanwhile, Monique Zinfani heard the doorbell ring. Looking at the camera feed, she saw a man
holding a large package that obscured his face and claiming to be the mailman. Mrs. Zinfani
buzzed the man in. Once he entered, the man dropped the package, produced a gun, and ordered
Mrs. Zinfani and a customer to the floor. Peter Zinfani, who was in the back of the showroom,
was able to run into the safe room where he pressed the alarm. However, the alarm was not
working and failed to notify the police.

While Mrs. Zinfani and the customer lay on the floor, the man in the postal uniform fumbled
with a black ski mask, eventually pulling it over his face, and then let his accomplice into the
house. Together, they quickly made off with $250,000 worth of jewelry and fled the scene.

The Investigation

The Manchester Police Department led the investigation but had few leads. The descriptions
provided by Mrs. Zinfani and Hauptmann were vague and included no identifying features.
Additionally, Mrs. Zinfani had only seen the first intruder for a few seconds as he entered the
house and fumbled with his mask.

Since U.S. postal worker Hauptmann had been abducted as part of the crime, much of the
evidence, including the mailed package that the intruder used to gain entry to the Zinfanis’

369
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

house, was funneled into a simultaneous federal investigation led by postal inspector Simon
Fontaine. Fontaine produced the first real suspect. According to Fontaine, an informant told him
that Alex Borrensa, a Caucasian, 21-year-old man with several drug arrests but no prior
convictions, was involved in the crime. Borrensa’s appearance did not differ significantly from
the descriptions of the first assailant given by Mrs. Zinfani and Hauptmann. On February 26,
1980, Fontaine arrived at the Zinfanis’ house to show Mrs. Zinfani a photo array that included
Borrensa’s photograph. Looking at the photo array, Mrs. Zinfani identified Borrensa as the first
assailant who gained access to her house and threatened her with a gun. Hauptmann was
subsequently shown the same photo array and identified another individual as the first assailant.
She stated she was “ninety percent sure” that the individual she selected was the perpetrator.
Fontaine relayed this information gained from his investigation to the Manchester police.

During the next two months, Manchester police tried to find additional information that would
connect Borrensa to the crime but were unable to uncover anything. On May 1, police showed
Mrs. Zinfani the same photo array that Fontaine had shown her, and she again chose Borrensa.
Following this second identification, police arrested Borrensa on June 3.

Police conducted an in-person line-up on August 31. Mrs. Zinfani was brought into the court to
make the identification and was walked past a group of men assembled for the line-up. She saw
Borrensa and spontaneously identified him as the first assailant. Garrett Martinez, the New
Hampshire Attorney General, and Rebecca Robinson, Assistant Attorney General, then indicted
Borrensa for armed robbery in the Hillsborough County Superior Court.

370
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

The Trial

At trial, the state’s case primarily rested on Monique Zinfani’s eyewitness identification
testimony and the information gained from Postal Inspector Fontaine’s informant. However,
because Fontaine argued that his informant's life would be in danger if he testified in open court,
and the prosecutors supported this argument, the judge agreed that Fontaine himself could testify
to what the informant said.

Borrensa’s public defender, Keith Millard, had little in terms of counterarguments. On January
15, 1980, the day of the crime, Borrensa admitted he had spent the day getting high on drugs
with acquaintances at a house were people often gathered to buy and use intravenous drugs.
Millard tried to locate these individuals to verify his client’s story, but Borrensa was unable to
provide any identifying information about his acquaintances. Millard also could not find anyone
who would admit to being at the house that day. Therefore, at trial Millard instead focused on
attacking Fontaine’s credibility and the credibility of his confidential source. Ultimately, the jury
rejected the defense’s argument, and Borrensa was convicted of armed robbery on February 20,
1981. He was sentenced to 15 years.

STOP:

Please answer #1-4 on the case evaluation sheet before reading the
remainder of the narrative.

371
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

RESUME:

Now please continue to read about the exoneration and answer #5-8 on the
case evaluation sheet.

The Exoneration
Borrensa served 13 years of his sentence before being released for good behavior in 1994. But by
1998, Borrensa was in trouble again, indicted for attempted murder of a mail carrier. The case
was under federal jurisdiction. As Borrensa’s federal public defender, Lila Anderson, prepared
for the trial, she received an unexpected phone call from a federal witness protection handler.

“Does your client ever mention a 1980 robbery conviction?” the caller asked Anderson.

“Yeah, he says he didn’t do it,” Anderson recalls responding.

“He’s right. He didn’t.”

Instead, the caller said that one of her charges, Billy Adler, a member of the notorious Oxman
Gang, had admitted numerous times over the last fifteen years to the armed robbery and gloated
about Borrensa taking the blame. Now, though, as Borrensa prepared to be tried for a second,
similar crime, the federal government was obligated to turn over this information as it impacted
sentencing.

372
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

After the call, Anderson, federal prosecutors, and the new New Hampshire Assistant Attorney
General Patrick DeStefano met to discuss the development. DeStefano asked Adler and his
handler to testify before a grand jury. Adler agreed and revealed details of the crime previously
not known. The judge commented upon the physical similarities between Borrensa and Adler,
saying they could almost be “brothers.” After the testimony, DeStefano petitioned the court to
vacate Borrensa’s earlier conviction and expunge his record, which was granted.

Additionally, DeStefano issued an internal investigation into Borrensa’s wrongful conviction,
including the reexamination of all state and federal evidence. Among those pieces tested was the
mail parcel used to gain access to the Zinfanis’ house. The new investigation found that it was
covered in fingerprints, which had been tested before Borrensa’s trial and did not match
Borrensa. The prints matched Adler. Fontaine, who would have been aware of the results, could
not explain why the prints had not been turned over to local police, the state prosecutor, or the
defense during the original trial. Police also tracked down Fontaine’s confidential informant,
who claimed he had never given Fontaine Borrensa’s name. When confronted with this denial,
Fontaine changed his story and claimed he actually received Borrensa’s name from a
confidential contact of his confidential informant. That contact was never found. In his final
report, DeStefano questioned whether the informant and contact had ever actually existed.
Borrensa agreed to a guilty plea for the new federal charge of attempted murder of a mail carrier.
He is currently serving a 25 year sentence.

Fontaine, who had retired, was not disciplined or prosecuted for his role in Borrensa’s case.

373
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.G.3. Erroneous Conviction Exemplar: Weak Prosecution and Probative Defense

Toby Jackson

The Crime

On August 1, 1981, Mary Mullen, a Caucasian 21-year-old Columbia University law student,
was found raped and murdered in Prospect Park in New York City, NY. Police theorized that
Mullen, who was employed as a paralegal by a downtown law firm, had been cutting through the
park after finishing work. Police suggested that Mullen’s assailant had attempted to rob her and
steal her purse, but when Mullen resisted her attacker turned violent, dragging Mullen from the
path, raping her, and then shooting her in the head five times. Despite their theory, Mullen’s
purse was found near her body undisturbed. Forensic evidence—pubic hair and semen—was
collected from Mullen’s body.

New York City Police (NYPD) almost immediately identified a likely suspect: Toby Jackson.
Jackson was a 28-year-old African-American Air Force enlistee who had previously attended the
University of Kansas. Recently, though, Jackson had attracted the negative attention of park and
city police. Approximately a month earlier, Jackson pled guilty to robbery for snatching a purse
from a female visitor to Prospect Park. Then, just two weeks after the purse snatching and two
weeks before Mullen’s murder, a young Caucasian woman was raped in Prospect Park. After
working with sketch artists, she created a composite image that reminded park police of Jackson.
Jackson was arrested on the rape charges, positively identified by the victim, and charged with

374
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

the rape. However, Jackson was able to provide an airtight alibi—he had spent the night of the
rape in jail for public drunkenness. Following verification of his alibi, the rape charges were
dropped.

Following the Mullen murder and rape, police suspected that Jackson might be involved. On
August 30, 1981, Jackson was arrested on unrelated misdemeanor charges, and his hair samples
were collected. These were sent for comparison to the FBI Forensic Laboratory. FBI Special
Agent Jenny Jonavitch confirmed that Jackson’s hairs matched those found on Mullen’s body.
Based on these results, Jackson was indicted for Mullen’s murder and rape. Steven Cabrini, who
was just preparing to retire, was appointed by the court to defend Jackson against the charges.
Cabrini immediately requested serology tests on the semen.

In preparing for the trial, Assistant District Attorney Alfonso Bryant authorized the use of a paid
informant. Mitt Yates had worked as a paid informant for the NYPD before. Police and
prosecutors encouraged Yates to engage Jackson in a conversation about Mullen and obtain a
confession. Eventually, Yates reported to his handler that Jackson had confessed, saying he had
raped and murdered a girl in Prospect Park. Bryant authorized a $1000 payment to Yates in
exchange for his work as a police informant.

Before the trial was to start in 1982, the serology results came back; the tests excluded Jackson
as the semen donor. Nonetheless, the prosecution continued.

375
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

The Trial

At trial, Bryant called both the informant, Yates, and Special Agent Jonavitch to the stand. Yates
testified that Jackson confessed to him while in prison. On cross, defense counsel Cabrini
attacked Yates’s credibility and the government’s payment. Special Agent Jonavitch explained
that a microscopic comparison matched Jackson’s hair and those found on Mullen’s body.
Jonavitch concluded that these hairs were microscopically identical. Additionally, Jonavitch
dismissed the recently returned serology results, claiming that the blood evidence had been
degraded. Thus, she contended that the absence of a match did not indicate Jackson was
innocent, just that the material was unsuitable for testing.

To respond to Jonavitch’s testimony, the defense called its own forensic expert. The defense
expert contested the reliability of microscopic hair comparison and disagreed with Jonavitch’s
interpretation of the serology results. However, when crossed by Bryant, the defense expert did
admit that a degraded sample could mask a positive serology match.

The prosecution also successfully petitioned for testimony about Jackson’s purse-snatching to be
permitted at trial. Bryant claimed the earlier act was “ambiguously sexual” in nature and
indicative of an escalating pattern in sexual violence. As a result, Bryant called the pursesnatching victim to the stand. She testified that Jackson had robbed her and made sexual remarks
in the process.

376
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

The defense attempted to introduce the earlier rape charges that were dismissed against Jackson.
The defense’s argument was that Jackson’s solid alibi and the police sketch showed that an
African-American man with a similar appearance to Jackson was raping young, Caucasian
women in Prospect Park and contributed to reasonable doubt. The judge denied the defense’s
motion.

The jury found Jackson guilty. He was sentenced to 20 years to life.

STOP:

Please answer #1-4 on the case evaluation sheet before reading the
remainder of the narrative.

377
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

RESUME:

Now please continue to read about the exoneration and answer #5-8 on the
case evaluation sheet.

Errors in Forensic Testimony

Nearly ten years later, in 1993, Dr. Hans Mueller, a former FBI Crime Lab analyst, revealed to
Congress that Jonavitch frequently falsified lab results and perjured testimony, usually in favor
of the prosecution. A 1997 report from the Department of Justice’s Office of the Inspector
General (“OIG”) confirmed that Jonavitch had falsified results in a number of cases, but did not
specifically examine the Jackson case. Regardless, Jackson’s defense was not notified of the
OIG’s findings. Following the 1997 OIG report, the Department of Justice formed a Brady Task
Force to investigate all cases Jonavitch had been involved in, including Jackson’s case. Though
the Task Force never issued an official report, the Department of Justice did notify the New York
District Attorney’s Office that Jonavitch’s testimony and final lab report in the Jackson case
were not supported by her notes. Indeed, Jonavitch’s notes suggested the blood sample was not
degraded and that the hair comparison did not show a microscopic match. The letter advised
prosecutors to determine if they needed to notify the defense as required under Brady, but the
defense was not informed of the Department of Justice’s letter.

378
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

Exoneration

In 2007, Jackson’s original defense attorney, Cabrini, wrote to the public defender’s office to
encourage them to reexamine the Jackson case in light of the FBI crime lab scandal. As a result,
public defender Amanda Bronson reopened Jackson’s case. She eventually found the Department
of Justice letter to the New York District Attorney’s Office. Upon learning that the original
testimony concerning the blood evidence was in error, Bronson worked with two police
detectives to track down the remaining forensic evidence from Jackson’s case. Eventually the
trio found that the original medical examiner had saved samples from the Jackson case. Retested
using new DNA tests, the results concluded that Jackson was not the semen donor.
On November 8, 2009, New York Supreme Court Judge Fenwick, the same man who had
originally sentenced Jackson to prison, exonerated him and declared him legally innocent in light
of the new forensic evidence. Jackson had served 27 years in prison. With the official declaration
of innocence, Jackson was eligible for $50,000 per year in compensation under New York law.
Jackson received approximately $1.4 million.

379
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.G.4. Near Miss Exemplar: Highly Probative Prosecution and Weak Defense

Carson Coleman

The Crime

Elaine Marsh had just settled down for bed around 8 p.m. on December 2, 2010 when the
doorbell rang. Marsh lived alone in an apartment in Kearney, NE. Although she had been
drinking and smoking pot, the 35-year-old, Caucasian woman answered the door. Through the
screen, she saw a man who did not identify himself but asked if Kirk, her ex-boyfriend, were
home. Marsh invited the man into her house, and told him her boyfriend was not there. She then
turned away, and as she did, the man grabbed her, dragged her into the living room, and forced
her to give him oral sex. He then raped her vaginally, dragged her to the bedroom and raped her
again. He also forcibly beat Marsh throughout the attack.

Over the next several hours, Marsh was raped multiple times. She testified that in between the
rapes, the assailant would leave her in the bedroom and go into her kitchen and drink. In total,
Marsh’s rapist drank an entire bottle of Vodka and ten Miller beers. She tried to escape twice,
once unsuccessfully. After 11 p.m., Marsh managed to escape and fled to her neighbor’s
apartment. From there, she watched her assailant and noticed he left with the remainder of the
pack of beer. Shortly afterwards, police responded.

380
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

The Identification

When asked about her rapist, Marsh directly implicated Carson Coleman, a friend of her exboyfriend. She had met Coleman just a few weeks before, though the encounter was brief, lasting
no more than a couple of hours at most. Still, she was sure Coleman was her attacker.

Coleman, a 27-year-old man34, was known to police. He had been arrested in 2002 for multiple
counts of burglary and had been arrested several times for public drunkenness. Police located
Coleman shortly after midnight about a block and a half from the victim’s apartment. He was
drinking Miller beer, the same type that had been stolen from the victim’s apartment. Police
arrested Coleman on a number of counts including knowingly concealing stolen property, rape,
forcible sodomy, robbery, and strangulation.

Concurrently, Marsh was taken to the hospital. Her physical injuries were photographed and
documented. However, because a Sexual Assault Nurse Examiner was not available, Marsh had
to wait two days before a rape kit could be collected. Marsh testified that during that time she
neither showered nor changed her clothes so that evidence was preserved.

Once the evidence was collected, the Buffalo County District Attorney requested it be sent to the
state forensic laboratory for testing. Coleman’s DNA was collected and sent as a comparison
sample.

                                                            
34

Coleman’s race and ethnicity are somewhat unclear. The Nebraska Department of Corrections website lists his
race as “Caucasian,” however, his photo, on the same site, clearly shows an individual who is Hispanic or Native
American.

381
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

STOP:

Please answer #1-4 on the case evaluation sheet before reading the
remainder of the narrative.

382
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

RESUME:

Now please continue to read about the exoneration and answer #5-8 on the
case evaluation sheet.

The Exoneration

Tests conducted at the state lab concluded that Coleman was not the donor of the DNA. In his
motion to dismiss the charges, District Attorney Jeremy Ambrose wrote:

“On this date, April 15, 2011, the State of Nebraska received confirmation
of results of DNA comparative analysis from NSBI [Nebraska State Bureau
of Investigation] pertaining to DNA evidence recovered from the person of
the alleged victim in this case. The results of said analysis proved to be
exculpatory and excluded the defendant as the doner [sic] of said DNA
evidence and accordingly the State moves to dismiss the charges. The
described exculpatory evidence was disclosed to counsel for the defense
upon confirmation and this motion tendered immediately thereafter.”

Coleman spent 134 days in the county jail, pretrial, after failing to post his bail due to financial
restraints. Coleman was released on April 19, 2011.

383
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.G.5. Near Miss Exemplar: Probative Prosecution and Probative Defense

Andre Towers

The Crime

On March 6, 2000 at approximately 7:30 a.m., Anne Rogawsky was fatally shot in the head by a
young man attempting to steal her purse. Rogawsky’s husband was with her on the sidewalk
when the incident occurred outside a Hampton Inn on Park Street in New Orleans, LA. The New
Orleans police were called and quickly arrived on the scene to speak with Mr. Rogawsky, a
middle-aged Caucasian tourist from Alabama. He described the shooter to police as a tall,
African-American male with skinny legs, in his mid-to-late twenties, and wearing a fishing style
hat. Mr. Rogawsky described the gun as a double barrel shotgun. Police also spoke briefly with
someone who had been eating at a nearby IHOP, who said he saw two African-American men
speeding away from the crime scene in a car.

Nearly two hours later, New Orleans police picked up 15-year-old African-American Lyon High
School student Andre Towers. Towers said he was on his way to Jiffy Lube to pick up a job
application at the time, and his appearance did not closely match Mr. Rogawsky’s description of
the shooter in age, height, or attire. Nonetheless, an officer approached Towers and asked him to
accompany the officer to the Hampton Inn to be of assistance in the investigation of a nearby
crime. Towers complied. The officer would later testify at trial that he picked up Towers because
he was an African-American male in the area close to the crime scene.

384
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

The police then conducted a show-up at the crime scene, and the victim’s husband identified
Andre Towers as the person who had shot his wife. Another person also identified Towers at the
scene as the African-American teenager who had been panhandling at the Hampton Inn the night
before the crime. After the identifications, police gave Towers a Gun Shot Residue test, and his
results were clean (indicating he had not recently shot a gun). The police then took him to the
station for questioning.

The Interrogation

At the station, Towers was held and interrogated for a total of over 12 hours without his parents
being notified of his whereabouts. Detective Oliver, who at the time had already been working
15 straight hours, was assigned to be one of the lead investigators. At the request of the New
Orleans State Attorney’s Office, none of the interrogation was recorded or videotaped. As a
result, the accounts of what took place during the interrogation would be highly disputed at trial.
Lead detectives Oliver and Sussey testified that Towers verbally confessed to the murder during
the first part of the interrogation, before Towers requested to call his parents, but Towers
adamantly refuted the claim. Towers insisted he maintained his innocence. Police declined his
offer to take a polygraph examination.

At 8:40 p.m., Towers asked to call his parents. The police did not call his parents and continued
to interrogate him. During this subsequent portion of his interrogation, Towers signed a
statement of admission and was arrested.

385
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

After the interrogation and arrest, police did not verify the statements given to them by Towers
(for instance, the victim’s husband described the murder weapon as a shotgun, while Towers
referred to it as a .38 during his confession), nor did they search the Towers family home. The
murder weapon was never found. However, the victim’s purse was recovered by a dumpster
diver the day after the crime on the opposite side of town.

Towers was indicted on first-degree murder charges and assigned two public defenders. In
preparation for trial, Towers signed an affidavit; in it, he alleged that in the first part of the
interrogation, Detective Sussey punched him, called him a racial epithet, threatened him with the
electric chair, and threatened to get someone to hurt his parents. Towers also alleged another
detective punched him in the eye and threatened to shoot him. Furthermore, Towers insisted that
he did not waive his rights voluntarily but was instead held forcefully by the chin until he signed
the waiver. Towers claimed that police then took him to the woods at dusk in an attempt to
recover the murder weapon. While Sergeant Johnson and Detective Oliver waited behind,
Detective Sussey took Towers into the woods. Towers maintained he was hit in the face and
punched twice in the stomach by Sussey after refusing to disclose the location of the weapon or
admit to the crime. Towers also stated that when he was taken back to the station for the final
portion of his interrogation, Detective Oliver continued the physical abuse, poking him in the eye
and unsnapping the holster of his gun to coerce Towers into signing the written admission.
Towers alleged Detective Oliver wrote most of the statement based upon what he wanted to hear,
and forced Towers to sign it.

386
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

The Trial

Towers’s trial began in September 2000. In a pretrial motion, defense counsel sought to suppress
the positive identification of Towers by the victim's husband and keep statements attributed to
Towers from being used in the trial. The Circuit Judge denied defense counsel's motion to
suppress the identification but agreed to withhold statements attributed to Towers that were made
after he asked to call his parents at 7:50 p.m. The judge held that the jury could hear oral
statements that police said Towers made to them prior to that time.

At trial, the state’s case rested on Towers’s confession and the identification made by the
victim’s husband at the crime scene. The prosecution also discussed the victim’s recovered
purse. The prosecutor explained that detectives failed to notify the prosecution that they had not
tested the victim’s purse for fingerprints until two weeks before trial. By then, there was not
enough time to run the tests and meet speedy trial deadlines. Therefore, no tests had been run.
The state did attempt to explain how Towers could have put the recovered purse in the dumpster
across town before police picked him up. The state argued he may have put it on the back of
tractor-trailer truck to get rid of it, and then the truck somehow dumped it, along with its load, in
the dumpster. However, the state offered no evidence to support this theory.

The defense focused on Towers’s allegations that his confession had been coerced. Defense
counsel presented a photo of the defendant with bruises on his face and argued the marks were
the direct result of the police’s violent interrogation. In addition, though he was only 15, his
parents were not notified of his whereabouts during the interrogation. Towers also testified that

387
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

he was intimidated and physically abused into confessing and reiterated all that was said in his
earlier affidavit. In addition, the defense argued it would have been impossible for Towers, who
was found near the crime scene and was in police custody less than two hours after the murder,
to have driven all the way across town with no car and license to dispose of the purse. Defense
counsel also accused police of racial profiling without thoroughly canvassing the area for
suspects.
STOP:

Please answer #1-4 on the case evaluation sheet before reading the
remainder of the narrative.

388
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

RESUME:

Now please continue to read about the exoneration and answer #5-8 on the
case evaluation sheet.

The Acquittal

The jury acquitted Towers after approximately one hour of deliberation. The jurors found
Towers’s testimony, along with the photo, to be critical in convincing them that the defendant
had been coerced into falsely confessing. However, Detective Sussey continued to deny the
allegations of misconduct against him, and State’s Attorney Patricia Rosengold maintained there
was no proof Towers had been physically abused during his interrogation.

The Exoneration

In early November 2000, a client of Towers’s former public defenders came to them and said he
had overheard a man named Bing Lopez talking about the Rogawsky case. The client said Lopez
had admitted over the phone that he committed the murder with the help of Cliff Johnson, who
served as the getaway driver. The public defenders went to the Sheriff’s Office with this new
information. The Sherriff, father of Detective Sussey, was initially reluctant to re-open the case,
but finally agreed.

When experts finally reviewed the fingerprints found on the victim’s recovered purse, they
concluded that the prints on the purse belonged to Bing Lopez. Three months later, police finally
located Lopez and Johnson. Lopez matched Mr. Rogawsky’s description of the killer and lived in

389
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

a building very close to the dumpster where the victim’s purse was recovered. Lopez’s criminal
record also included a robbery similar to what happened the day of the slaying. Johnson had been
arrested about an hour after the shooting on a driver's license violation after running a stop sign
across town, but he was not initially linked to the slaying. Police arrested and charged both men
with murder. Johnson pled guilty to second-degree murder in the case and was sentenced to ten
years in prison after he agreed to testify against Lopez at trial. Lopez was convicted of firstdegree murder in 2001 and sentenced to life in prison.

In light of the new evidence, on January 3, 2001, the State’s Attorney took the unusual step of
publicly apologizing to Towers for admittedly being too rash to charge him with the crime. As
the Louisiana Times reported in January 2001, Rosengold said words could not express the
“regret of [her] entire office for the ordeal Mr. Towers and his family wrongly endured.” Police
and prosecutors said they had no doubt that Towers was innocent, and that the testimony of the
victim’s husband Mr. Rogawsky had been mistaken. Miriam Foster, the foreman of the jury that
acquitted Towers, said police and prosecutors should have apologized to Towers long ago.

The governor appointed a special prosecutor to lead a grand jury probe into the actions of the
police and prosecutors in the Towers case. Although the grand jury found no evidence of
criminal conduct, they were critical of the individuals and offices involved and recognized that
the evidence against Towers was not thoroughly reviewed before his indictment.
Towers’s family filed a civil suit against the city of New Orleans in 2001, seeking $8.3 million in
damages. They settled in March 2002 for $780,000, citing the length of time the case would drag

390
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

on and the difficulty in proving the case under federal civil rights law as reasons for the
settlement.

391
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.G.6. Near Miss Exemplar: Weak Prosecution and Highly Probative Defense

Michael Kincaid

The Crime

Between 4 and 6 a.m. on the morning of May 24, 1981, Kara Lewis, a 12-year-old AfricanAmerican girl, was raped and bludgeoned to death, and her 10-year-old brother Jamal was beaten
unconscious. The crime occurred in their home in a poor neighborhood of Detroit, MI.
When the Detroit Police Department began investigating, they found a blood-stained pipe in an
alley behind the Lewis home. In addition, a neighbor, Jenny Baker, reported seeing an AfricanAmerican teenager leaving the alley at about 6 a.m., clad in a dark knee-length coat and a hat and
carrying a red tote bag.

On May 31, a week after the crime, police went to the hospital to talk to Jamal, who was finally
able to speak with difficulty and respond to simple verbal cues. The detectives attempted to
interview him, but Jamal found it difficult to talk, so the officers told him to signal "yes" or "no"
by squeezing the officer’s hand. Jamal uttered the name "Michael" and then through hand signals
conveyed the message that "Michael" was the assailant and that Michael was a teenage gang
member who lived near the Lewis home and had lighter skin than Jamal.

392
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

The Pre-Arrest Identification

Based on this interview, police began looking for persons who lived near the Lewis home and fit
the description of "Michael." They discovered that Michael Kincaid lived a block away. An
African-American, 18-year-old Metro High School senior and the editor of the school
newspaper, Kincaid was called "the Professor" by his classmates due to his studious character.
The police obtained a photograph of Kincaid -- his graduation photograph, which showed him in
a suit -- and created a photo array with this photo and various mug shots.

Police took the photos to show Jamal. Moments before the officers spoke to Jamal, he overheard
that his sister was dead and became agitated. The officers knew this, but proceeded with the
photo line-up. They displayed Kincaid's picture first and asked Jamal whether he knew the
person. He said yes, and the police then asked him whether this person was the assailant. Jamal
did not respond. He remained unresponsive after being shown the rest of the pictures. In
addition, just before the interview, Mrs. Lewis had told the officers that Jamal had been
murmuring a name that sounded like "Michael Watson," "Michael Watkins," or "Michael
Walton." In the interview, police heard Jamal repeat a name that sounded like "Watson."

After this second interview with Jamal, police tried to find a "Watson" in the Lewis
neighborhood, but were unsuccessful. When the officers wrote up their memorandum on the
progress of the case, they placed copies in the police department's "street files." These were files
that the police did not turn over to the state's attorney's office as they did with their regular

393
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

investigative files. As a result, the street files were not available to defense counsel even if they
contained exculpatory material.

The next morning the police went back to the hospital and repeated the photo line-up. Jamal was
shown the pictures one at a time, starting with Kincaid's picture. Jamal made no response to it
but upon seeing one of the subsequent pictures of another individual he cried out "Yep, yep,
that's the one who did it to me." Police asked Jamal whether he knew the person's name; he made
no response. He also made no response when asked whether he knew the person's nickname and
whether it was "Professor."

The Post-Arrest Identification

After this third interview, police found Kincaid at his high school and arrested him. They
searched his locker but did not find any clothes or bag that matched Jenny Baker’s description.
They took him to the police station, where they questioned him and threatened him with the
electric chair if he didn't confess. He denied having anything to do with the crime and was sent to
the County Jail.

Kincaid's graduation photo was subsequently shown to Jenny Baker, who at first was uncertain.
But after covering up part of the face with strips of paper, she said he was indeed the "clean
looking" person she had seen emerging from the alley on the morning of the crime.

394
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

Accompanied by two assistant state's attorneys, the police brought Kincaid to Jamal's hospital
room, ordered him to stand about three and a half feet from the bed, and asked Jamal whether
this was the person who had hit him over the head. Speaking calmly, Jamal answered, "No, that's
not the man, that's not the man, no, no, no." An officer ordered Kincaid to take off his glasses
and stand within two feet of Jamal. He turned up the lights, then repeated the question. "No,"
said Jamal -- then, "Yes, that's him, yes"; then, "Yes, no, yes, no" over and over.

The morning after this "identification," a grand jury indicted Michael Kincaid for murder, rape,
attempted murder, armed violence, burglary, and home invasion. On the same day (June 6),
police signed the official arrest report, which was full of falsehoods. For instance, it stated that:
Jenny Baker had picked out Kincaid's picture from a group of seven (his picture was the only one
she had been shown), Kincaid's father had not seen him on the morning of May 24 (in fact his
father had told the investigating officers that he had seen Michael at home that morning), and
Jamal had said that his assailant attended Metro High School (Jamal had said no such thing). The
report did not mention that Jamal had described his assailant as a gang member (Kincaid was not
a gang member), as being a lighter-skinned African-American than Jamal (Kincaid is darkerskinned than Jamal), and as having a name like Watson. The report also did not mention that the
doctors had warned the officers at least twice that Jamal's head injury had left him with serious
memory problems.

395
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

New Evidence

On June 14, Mrs. Lewis reported to the police that she had found two pairs of pantyhose in her
house that belonged neither to her nor to her murdered daughter. A new officer, Detective
O’Brien, was assigned to investigate this evidence. O’Brien went to Jamal, who was still in the
hospital, and asked him what he knew about the pantyhose. Jamal said for the first time that there
had been two assailants and that both had worn stocking masks. Jamal referred to one of the
assailants as Michael Watson and to the other as "twin brother Michael." He reiterated that
Michael Watson was a gang member. O’Brien then went to his superiors and told him he was
convinced the wrong person had been charged. His superiors told him to continue investigating
but took no other action.

After O’Brien’s visit, two other officers went to the hospital to interview Jamal yet again. Jamal
repeated that there had been two assailants, both masked. But this time Jamal added that one of
them had removed his mask during the assault. He was not asked to describe that assailant.
The final official report omitted any reference to O’Brien and concluded that no further
investigation was warranted. O’Brien wrote up his interview with Jamal as a supplement to the
previous police reports in the case, but was told to make it an unofficial memo. The memo was
placed in the street files.

396
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

A New Suspect

In August of the same year, 21-year-old Cynthia Marx was raped and bludgeoned to death, four
blocks from the Lewis home. Jeremy Fuller was arrested and confessed to the crime. O’Brien
thought Fuller matched Jamal's description of his attacker: Fuller went by the name "King
Michael," was a gang member, and had lighter skin than Jamal. Questioned by O’Brien about the
Lewis crime, Fuller said that it was possible he had committed it, but that as he had frequent
blackouts, he wasn't sure. O’Brien reported this information to his superiors and asked to arrange
for Jamal to view a line-up with Fuller in it. They refused on the ground that Jamal was not
competent to make an identification, adding that because of Jamal's incompetence the
prosecution of Kincaid had been abandoned. In fact, the case was still proceeding to trial.
O’Brien filed a report opining that Fuller, not Kincaid, had committed the Lewis crime. The
report was never forwarded to the state's attorney's office.

Forensic Evidence

In October, in preparation for trial, police laboratory technician Donna Ambrose analyzed the
semen found in Cynthia Marx’s vagina and determined that Kincaid had a different blood type
than the contributor of the semen. Ambrose failed to include this information in the lab report
that she was preparing for the prosecution. Kincaid's lawyers called Ambrose after her first
report and asked about the blood results, so Ambrose subsequently issued a second report (which
apparently was made known to both the prosecution and defense) with her findings that excluded
Kincaid as the contributor of semen. Ambrose later testified that she "inadvertently" left things

397
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

out of both reports. Additionally, she placed in the Cynthia Marx file, but not in the Lewis file,
the results of an examination of hair taken from the pantyhose that Mrs. Lewis had discovered.
This examination indicated that one of the hairs found was not Kincaid's but might be Fuller's.
The other hairs found in the pantyhose could not be tested.

STOP:

Please answer #1-4 on the case evaluation sheet before reading the
remainder of the narrative.

398
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

RESUME:

Now please continue to read about the exoneration and answer #5-8 on the
case evaluation sheet.

The Exoneration

Kincaid’s trial began in the spring of 1982. When Detective O’Brien read about the trial in the
newspaper, he was astonished because he had been told the prosecution was abandoned. He
approached his superiors and reiterated his belief that an innocent boy was being falsely
prosecuted; however, his superiors again declined to reopen the case. O’Brien then told Kincaid's
lawyer about the exculpatory information secreted in the "street files." The lawyer promptly
relayed O’Brien's disclosures to the judge, who declared a mistrial. Shortly afterward, the state's
attorney dropped all charges against Kincaid.

Kincaid sued the City of Detroit, several Detroit police officers and Donna Ambrose, the crime
lab technician, for false arrest, false imprisonment, intentional infliction of emotional distress,
and malicious prosecution, as well as conspiracy to commit these wrongs. On October 2, 1988 a
jury awarded him $801,000 in compensatory and punitive damages. The defendants appealed but
the U.S. Court of Appeals found sufficient evidence of conspiracy and wrongdoing on the part of
the City, its officers, and the technician to uphold the verdict.35

                                                            
35

Most of the facts in this narrative are taken from the U.S. Court of Appeals’ opinion. The court was viewing the
facts in the light most favorable to Kincaid, but it found sufficient evidence for a jury to conclude that this version of
the facts, and the wrongdoing it implies, is true.

399
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.H. Case Evaluation Sheet for Expert Panelists

Name of Panelist______________________
DEFENDANT’S NAME________________________________________________________
CONVICTION or INDICTMENT (circle one)

1. IN YOUR OPINION, WHAT EVIDENCE OF GUILT LED TO THE CONVICTION OR
INDICTMENT?
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________

2. HOW STRONG WAS THE STATE’S EVIDENCE IN SUGGESTING THE DEFENDANT’S
GUILT? (assign a number on a 1-3 scale to each type of evidence, 1 being weak evidence, 3
being strong evidence; elaborate if desired)

EYEWITNESS IDENTIFICATION:_______________________________________
CONFESSION/INCRIMINATING STATEMENTS:___________________________
NONEYEWITNESS TESTIMONY:______________________________________

400
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

FORENSIC EVIDENCE:______________________________________________
OTHER EVIDENCE:________________________________________________
TOTAL EVIDENCE:_________________________________________________

3. HOW STRONG WAS THE DEFENSE CASE? (assign a number on a 1-3 scale, 1 being
weak, 3 being strong; elaborate if desired; if there was no discernible defense case, mark as N/A)
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________

4. HOW REASONABLE WAS THE CONVICTION OR INDICTMENT AND WHY?
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
________________________________________________________________

401
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

5. IN YOUR OPINION, WHAT ERRORS, IF ANY, LED TO THE CONVICTION OR
INDICTMENT? (when appropriate, include errors made by both the state and defense)
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________

6. FOR WRONGFUL CONVICTIONS ONLY, WHAT EVIDENCE LED TO THE
DEFENDANT’S EXONERATION AND WHO WAS RESPONSIBLE?
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________

7. FOR DISMISSALS ONLY, WHAT EVIDENCE, IF ANY, JUSTIFIED THE DISMISSAL IN
YOUR OPINION?
______________________________________________________________________________

402
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________

8. WHAT, IF ANYTHING, COULD HAVE BEEN DONE IN THIS CASE TO PREVENT THE
WRONGFUL CONVICTION OR
INDICTMENT?________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
______________________________________________________________________________
____________________________________________________________________________

403
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

VIII.I. Technical Appendix
VIII.I.A. Multiple Imputation
Imputation is the process of filling missing data with plausible values. Rubin (1977)
outlines the problems of simply filling in the mean of a series or some other naïve way to replace
the missing value and argues for multiple imputation. Multiple imputation is a simulation
technique where the missing values are replaced with m>1 versions of the data to reflect
uncertainty in the estimates.36 As King et al. (2001) note, methodologists and statisticians are
nearly unanimous in their agreement that multiple imputation is a better technique to deal with
missing data than the conventional applied data analysis approach—listwise deletion. When data
are missing for the independent variables or dependent variable in many datasets, the entire case
is thrown out.37 If there are patterns of missing data across multiple variables, then a large
portion of the cases will simply be discarded.
There are three reasons these data can be missing.38 First, they can be missing completely
at random (MCAR). If data are missing for this reason, then information related to the missing
values will not help predict the missing values. Second, data can be missing at random (MAR).
In MAR data, observable information can help predict the missing value. For example, if
women tend to finish high school more often than men, we can use gender to predict the
likelihood that a defendant has completed high school. The more variables that aid in the
prediction, the better the estimate. Third, data can be nonignorable (NI) or missing due to
unobserved factors. These data cannot be recovered from available information. For example, if
                                                            
36

The standard is to use five imputations, but any number can be used. Rubin (1987) suggests anywhere from two to
ten imputations are sufficient for most applications. We use five but also use three and four as robustness checks
and do not find any substantive difference in the results.
37
We perform imputations for only a subset of the independent variables and not for the dependent variable.
38
This discussion of the three types of missing data is informed by King et al. (2001). See this piece for a more
technical account of these distinctions.

404
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

finishing high school is almost entirely related to the aptitude of the defendant, this may be
difficult to directly observe and thus imputation using observable factors will not improve our
predictions of high school completion.
The primary benefit of multiple imputation is retention of case data; thus, making
inferences is more efficient than listwise deletion (King et al., 2001). Additionally, the
inferences from multiple imputation are not biased under the MCAR and MAR assumptions
(Little & Rubin, 1989; King et al., 2001). If data are missing because of unobservable factors
(NI), then multiple imputation will not reduce the bias in the estimate but it will still likely be
more efficient than listwise deletion. In sum, multiple imputation is no worse than listwise
deletion and almost always an improvement.39
To impute our project data, we used a chained imputation procedure outlined by Royston
(2004). Chained imputation allows for imputation in a row of data that might have multiple
missing values and can be used when many variables are used as predictors (Van Buuren et al.,
1999). It involves a variable-by-variable approach that involves all other variables as predictors
(Horton & Kleinman, 2007). Continuous variables are estimated using OLS regression, counts
using a negative binomial estimator, and dichotomous variables using a logit/probit model. In
Horton and Kleinman’s (2007) simulated comparison of multiple imputation methods, the
chained equation technique used here performed as well as most other procedures in reducing
both bias in the coefficient estimates and standard errors. We used logit models to predict the
series of dichotomous variables. Since our continuous or near continuous measures of Death
Penalty Culture (Death Penalty Culture1, Death Penalty Culture2, Punitive Rank)40 are bounded

                                                            
39

See Shafer (1997) for a nontechnical overview of multiple imputation.
Death Penalty Culture 1 is constructed as the total number of executions per state divided by population in a given
year. Death Penalty Culture 2 is the total number of executions per state divided by the total number of homicides
40

405
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

(see Table 30), we used a truncated regression to limit the possible values to those that appear in
the actual data. Since Age of Defendant is a count, we use a negative binomial regression model
to impute its values. To impute the Logged Time from Arrest to Indictment, (days and months)
we use a linear regression. As Rubin (1996) makes clear, these imputations do not have to be
exactly like the observed data. Table 31 shows the summary statistics of the observed data.
The role of the imputed values should be to result in data that allow for valid statistical
inference. To do this, we use as much information as we can (all of the variables for which we
have complete data) and use estimators for imputation that can approximate the data generating
process.
The chained imputation models generate five imputed datasets. Table 32 provides
summary statistics for the observed and complete data (summary statistics of the combined
datasets).41 Figures 5 and 6 show the observed, imputed and complete (observed + imputed) data
densities. These figures show that the imputed data for these measures are not exactly the same
as the observed data but approximate the mean and standard deviations, and have little effect on
the distributions of the final series.42

VIII.I.B. Receiver-Operating Characteristic Curves
Evaluating model fit in binary dependent variable models is different than ordinary least
squares regression. Since the dependent variable only has two potential outcomes, Aldrich and
Forrest (1984: 15) claim that the “use of the coefficient of determination [R2] as a summary
statistic should be avoided.” Different pseudo R2 measures have been generated to approximate
                                                                                                                                                                                                
in a given year. The Rank variable is based on a rank of state punitiveness developed by Kutateladze (2009); see
footnote 15.
41
We use Klein’s misum command for Stata to generate these summaries.
42
Thanks to Wes Eddings for providing the Stata routine midiagplots that generates these figures (Marchenko and
Eddings 2011).

406
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

this statistic that reports explained variation. Since these models are calculated using maximum
likelihood, they are quite different than what is reported in an ordinary least squares model.
Each of these pseudo measures can generate substantially different values, thus limiting their
utility in assessing model fit (Long & Freese, 2006).
Most scholars instead use one of three alternatives: percent correctly predicted (PcP), the
percent reduction in error (PRE), or the Receiver-Operating Characteristic (ROC) curve. The
PcP examines each case of the model and compares the true outcome (in this project whether the
case was an erroneous conviction (1) or a near miss (0)) to the expected outcome given the
prediction of the model. Since this is a predicted probability, this number will range from zero to
one. If the prediction is above 0.5, then the observation is coded as a likely erroneous conviction
(1). If it is below 0.5, it is coded as a near miss (0). Then, the actual value of the dependent
variable is compared. The percentage of cases correctly predicted then gives the analyst a
summary statistic for the model fit (in-sample). The problem with the PcP approach is that it
treats a prediction of 0.4999 as a 0, but a prediction of a 0.5001 as a 1. Moreover, if the
threshold for what is considered a 1 changes, the PcP can change.
The PRE is a measure of the reduction in error based on our model when examining our
guesses of 1s and 0s as compared to a naïve guess. A naïve guess is just assuming the outcome
of the dependent variable is its modal category. If, for example, we know that 56.5% of our
cases are erroneous convictions, then guessing erroneous conviction for each case would lead to
us being correct 56.5% of the time (and wrong 43.5% of the time). The PRE is calculated by
adding up our model’s successful guesses of zeroes and ones and then comparing this to the
naïve model. The final number is a percentage improvement in our accuracy. Again, this
measure is sensitive to the threshold we use to establish a 1.

407
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

Fortunately, the Receiver-Operating Characteristic (ROC) curve can remedy the
shortcomings of the PRE and PcP. The ROC curve provides a graphical display of how well the
model predicts true positives, or sensitivity, compared to false positives, or 1-specificity. As we
increase the threshold for what is considered a positive, it should cut down on false positives but
may cut down on true positives as well (Beck et al., 2001). The ROC curve then can provide a
summary of the predictive ability of the model while adjusting the threshold for what is
considered a positive outcome.
Figure 2 offers an example ROC curve. This is a curve generated after estimating a
simple model using the control variables and nature of the crime measures (see Table 24). The Y
axis is the sensitivity, or the true positive rate, and the X axis is the 1-specificity, or the false
positive rate. The 45 degree line represents the 50/50 probability of a dichotomous outcome (if
we used a coin flip to predict the dependent variable). As the ROC curve extends beyond this 45
degree line, we are better able to predict true and false positives (beyond a coin flip). The area
under the ROC curve and above the 45 degree line gives a unique measure of model fit.

[Figure 2 about here]

In Figure 2, this area under the ROC curve is 0.816. In short, this model accurately
predicts about 82% of the 1s and 0s given varying thresholds for what is considered a positive
prediction. In the models that we estimate, we use this area under the ROC curve statistic as the
measure of model fit. While there is not a standard for what this number should be, between 80 -

408
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

89% is generally considered a good model and above 90% is an excellent model at predicting the
outcome of interest.43

VIII.I.C. Prediction/Forecasting
As the ROC curves show, the statistical model is able to predict erroneous convictions at
a high rate (0.908) (Figure 3). Nearly 91% of the time, the model can accurately predict an
erroneous conviction versus a near miss. Even in the more simple models (Tables 24-28), the
area under the ROC curve is between 0.801 and 0.827. While this evidence suggests that the
models are useful, there is a concern with overfitting. In short, when data are used to estimate a
model, then the fit we find may be particular to these data. The model may be good at predicting
outcomes in the same data used to estimate the model (in-sample prediction) but we do not know
how the predictive ability of the model will apply to other datasets (out-of-sample prediction).
Ideally, we would estimate this model and then examine whether the relationships and
estimates hold when using new data. Given the difficulty and time it took to collect the current
set of cases that meet our definition, it seems unlikely that we could find a new sample to
estimate another model. Fortunately, statisticians have developed a procedure called crossvalidation to perform this task. There are many ways to do cross-validation, but the intuition of
all of these algorithms is the same. First, the analyst divides the data into a certain number of
groups (we use four groups, for example). The groups are then divided into two sets: a training
set and a test set. The training set trains the model. These data are used to establish estimates
for the model. The results are then tested on the test set. In sum, the test set is not used in the
                                                            
43

See http://gim.unmc.edu/dxtests/roc3.htm and http://www.cpdm.ufpr.br/documentos/ROC.pdf for discussions of
this rule of thumb.

409
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.

 

training of the model and thus the predictions of the test set are out of sample. In this way, crossvalidation can avoid the criticism of overfitting and allow for an out-of-sample test of the model.
Second, this procedure is replicated for all of the groups in the data. With four groups, each has
a turn to be the test set. This way, the results should not be sensitive to how the data are divided.
Third, the results are then averaged across the groups. This again should average out any
divergent outcomes caused by an arbitrary division of the data. Finally, this process is repeated n
number of times (we used ten iterations for each data set).
Usually more of the data is used to train the model, and less is used to test the model. In
our case, we used three groups to train the model and one group to test the model. In our data,
we randomly assigned a group of data to the test (115 observations out of 460) or training set
(345 observations out of 460). We had each group serve as the test group and then predicted an
ROC curve for the group not used to train the model. This process was done ten times for each
data set. We performed four tests per division ten times for each data set (40 models times five
data sets equals 200 total out of sample forecasts of the ROC curve).44 We then averaged the
results of the ROC curves for each dataset to attain a final out of sample ROC curve estimate of
0.8709. In sum, our model is able to predict over 87% of the cases of erroneous convictions out
of sample. This is a slight decrease as compared to the in-sample forecasts (91%) but the
forecasts are still high, suggesting overfitting is not a serious problem with the model. We
expect that this model could be used to predict future cases across the United States that are
likely to be erroneous convictions with a high degree of accuracy.

 
                                                            
44

This is a procedure called K(4)-Fold Cross-Validation. See Ward et al. (2010) for a complete description.

410
This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not
been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s)
and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice.